Volume 15, Issue 4 (July & August 2024)                   BCN 2024, 15(4): 531-540 | Back to browse issues page


XML Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Mahjoubnavaz F, Khosrowabadi E, Latifi S, Daroughe Kazem Y, Gholizadeh Soltani J, Khalilpour H et al . Effect of Low-intensity Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation on Response Inhibition of Adults With Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. BCN 2024; 15 (4) :531-540
URL: http://bcn.iums.ac.ir/article-1-2558-en.html
1- Department of Cognitive Psychology, Institute for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran.
2- Neuroscience Research Center, School of Medicine, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran.
3- Institute for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran.
4- Department of Clinical Psychology, School of Behavioral Sciences, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran.
5- Alborz University of Medical Sciences, Karaj, Iran.
6- Islamic Azad University, Saveh Branch, Saveh, Iran.
7- Department of Cognitive Modeling, Institute for Cognitive Science Studies, Tehran, Iran.
Full-Text [PDF 1635 kb]       |   Abstract (HTML) 
Full-Text:  
1. Introduction 
Executive functions include various cognitive utilities, such as planning, working memory, attention, and response inhibition. Among them, response inhibition helps an individual avoid pre-planned responses and delay a response while not interfering with other cognitive functions. This function plays an important role in social interactions. Failure in response inhibition causes an inability to sustain attention, be easily distracted, and control behavior. In addition, dysfunction of the response inhibition forces a person to respond to a stimulus before correctly understanding the desired stimulus or making mistakes in searching for a desired goal among the stimuli due to disturbing stimuli. Deficiency in response inhibition can be seen in disorders such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and obsessive-compulsive disorder (Garavan et al., 1999; Gorfein & MacLeod, 2007; Harnishfeger & Bjorklund, 1993; Tamm et al., 2002).
ADHD is introduced as a neurodevelopmental disorder associated with environmental and genetic factors. ADHD is characterized by three important indicators: attention deficit, hyperactivity, and impulsivity. It was previously believed that ADHD appears only in childhood and improves during adolescence and adulthood, but recent studies showed that ADHD can continue into adulthood and causes many problems, including deficits in executive functions and social relationships (Barbaresi et al., 2002; Cuffe et al., 2001; DuPaul et al., 1991; Neuman et al., 2005). One of the main problems in the cognitive functioning of ADHD individuals is correct response inhibition. Due to their impulsive behavior, ADHD individuals cannot inhibit a pre-planned (dominant) response and focus on the task. Studies show that ADHD people have a lower response inhibition compared to normal individuals while executing a Stroop color and word test (SCWT) (Barkley, 1997; Barkley, 2000; Fischer et al., 2005; King et al., 2007; Nigg, 2001; Song & Hakoda, 2011). In addition, a lack of proper functioning in response inhibition causes ADHD people to miss the necessary patience in their demands. 
Since these cognitive deficits are linked to dysfunction of some brain areas, including the frontal lobe, striatum, and cerebellum, various pharmaceutical and non-pharmaceutical interventions have been introduced. Each method may improve the problem, but non-pharmaceutical treatments, including transcranial electrical and magnetic stimulations, have received more attention due to the lack of side effects (Breitling et al., 2020; Cosmo et al., 2020). The noninvasive brain stimulation (NIBS) techniques have been effectively used in recent years, and their capability to improve cognitive functions in various mental disorders, including ADHD, has been presented (Acosta & Leon-Sarmiento, 2003; Acosta et al., 2002; Hallett, 2001). Several brain areas showed the potential of targeting NIBS in ADHD people, including stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) for improvement of attention functions and response inhibition (Blasi et al., 2006), the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) for improvement of emotional regulation, and the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (DACC) for multiple attention and cognitive control. 
Considering the mechanism of response inhibition and the functional role of the DLPFC, the target site of NIBS should reduce the number of errors in ADHD people (Barkley, 1997; Croarkin et al., 2011; Jiang et al., 2018; Ridding & Rothwell, 2007; Willcutt et al., 2005).
Studies have reported that electrical stimulation of the DLPFC can improve executive functions in ADHD people (Friehs et al., 2021), even in a single-session stimulation (Dubreuil-Vall et al., 2020). In addition, the effectiveness of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) on the DLPFC region has also been discussed (Zaman, 2015), and various parameters such as frequency, intensity, duration of stimulation, and the interval between stimulations have been investigated (Tang et al., 2018). Nonetheless, rTMS has a low spatial resolution between 10 to 30 mm, and the low-intensity magnetic stimulation method has been proposed to be more intensive and improve the spatial resolution to a range between 1 and 5 mm (Colella et al., 2019).
The low-intensity magnetic stimulation is one of the most influential and precise magnetic stimulation methods. Also, its desired effects for improving visual attention (Grosbras & Paus, 2002; Heinen et al., 2014) and treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder by stimulating the prefrontal areas have been indicated (Boggio et al., 2010). Moreover, it has been shown that effective stimulation of the visual cortex can improve the perception of poor visual stimuli that cannot be perceived unconsciously (Abrahamyan et al., 2015). Therefore, we hypothesized that focal stimulation of DLPFC using the micro-TMS approach could be an effective method to improve response inhibition in ADHD individuals. Hence, we aimed to investigate micro-TMS’s effectiveness in enhancing response inhibition in ADHD patients while performing the SCWT in a single-session double-blind paradigm.

2. Materials and Methods

Participants and apparatus

Twenty-two sessions of recording were performed for 11 male adults with ADHD (age range: 18-36 years). All participants had a bachelor’s or a higher university degree. The inclusion criteria were diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders fifth edition (DSM-5), tests and clinical interviews. This study was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration. In addition, all participants were asked to complete and sign the consent form before entering the study. 

Stimuli
Several tests have been introduced to measure response inhibition in ADHD individuals. One of these tests is the SCWT, which measures various parameters by considering the type of response and the duration of the reactions in responding to a group of congruent–incongruent stimuli. This test was first designed and introduced by Ridley Stroop in 1935 to measure the level of attention. The computer-based version of the SCWT test used in the present study consisted of two categories: congruent (matching the word’s color with the word’s meaning) and incongruent (mismatching of the word’s color with the word’s meaning) events. The accuracy and validity of the Persian test have been reported to be 80% and 91%, respectively (Khodadadi et al., 2014). The duration of SCWT stimuli was 2000 ms with an interval of 800 ms.
The micro-TMS stimulation was performed using the Beta 1 device by the Parseh-Sanat-Ahouraian Company. The stimulation was performed using an intensity of 140 micro-tesla at a frequency of 17 Hz. The sham stimulation was also performed only by putting the related electrode on the target scalp position, and no stimulation was performed.
The participants’ brain activities were also recorded using the 19 electrodes placed on the scalp based on the international 10-20 standard arrangement. EEG data were recorded with a sampling rate of 500 Hz using an amplifier made by the LIV technology company, and a reference electrode was placed on the right ear. 

Experimental procedure
This study used a control, sham, and real stimulation paradigm. All participants were asked to perform a computer-based SCWT three times. The participants’ brain activities were recorded before the experiment started, as well as before and after the stimulations. The third examiner randomly selected the sequence of the sham or the real stimulation, and participants had 30 minutes of rest after each run of the task. The SCWT test was presented on a 22-inch desktop monitor placed 1 m away from the subject, and the participants answered the test by pressing the arrow key on a QWERTY keyboard.
The participants were asked to sit on a comfort chair in front of a 22-inch monitor, and an EEG cap was placed on the volunteer’s head. After calibrating the EEG recording device, EEG was recorded from each candidate in a resting state during eyes open and closed conditions for two minutes. Then, the participants were asked to perform the SCWT and had a 30-minute rest. Subsequently, the EEG recordings were performed again, and the participants were asked to repeat the test while a sham/real micro-TMS was applied to the right DLPFC.
To evaluate the results of this study, criteria such as number of errors, number of correct answers, and reaction times for answering were measured. Consequently, participants’ performance at the three phases of the conducted SCWT for 20 minutes was evaluated. The first stage was done before stimulation; the second and third stages were done after sham or real stimulation using micro-TMS. 
A standard preprocessing pipeline was performed on the EEG data, including bandpass filtering 1-40 Hz, running independent component analysis, removing a noisy component, visual inspection of the cleaned data, and re-referencing the data to the average of all channels. Subsequently, using the fast Fourier transform analysis, an absolute power of the cleaned EEG data was calculated in the conventional frequency bands. Then, the ratio between each band’s powers and the total band’s amount was calculated to point to the relative power of each frequency band.

Statistical analysis
To compare the effect of stimulation on response inhibition, the number of errors, correct answers, missed answers, and reaction times were computed in three phases of the experiments. In addition, comparisons were made in both congruent and incongruent words. After the test of normality using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, a paired t-test was done to compare the behavioral and relative powers of the two desired conditions. Lastly, the results of EEG data analysis were corrected for multiple comparison effects using the false discovery rate method.

3. Results
The results of this study were categorized into behavioral and neurophysiological findings. In the category of behavioral data analysis of the congruent color words, comparisons between real stimulation and control (no stimulation) and sham stimulation using paired-wise t-test showed that error responses were significantly lower in the real condition as compared to the control condition (P=0.04, t=-2.32) and slightly improved as compared to sham stimulation (P=0.13, t=-1.61), with a faster reaction time (P=0.03, t=-2.43) compared to the control condition, no significant difference with sham stimulation.
While for the incongruent color word, comparisons between real stimulation and control (no stimulation or sham stimulation) showed that error responses were only significantly lower in the sham condition (P=0.01, t=-2.79 as compared to the control condition), with a faster reaction time (P=0.02, t=-2.64). Interestingly, no significant differences were observed for other parameters, including missing, wrong, and correct answers. However, the results of real stimulation had a similar trend, but no significant result was observed (error responses in real stimulation compared to the control condition, P=0.07, t=-2.02).
Regarding the neurophysiology data, statistical analysis of EEG data showed that the spectral power of delta band frequencies at the frontal regions was significantly changed after real stimulation compared to the sham stimulation (Table 1). In addition, the power of brain waves at the beta band frequencies and its sub-bands (beta1 and beta) were also significantly changed in the prefrontal and parietal regions (Table 1).



Changes in spectral power of delta band activities in resting-state EEG data during eyes closed condition showed significant differences between real and sham stimulation at the F8 channel (P=0.03, t=-2.37).
Significant changes in the resting state EEG of the eyes open condition (Figure 1) are observed in delta band frequencies at the FP1 channel. The real micro-TMS stimulation caused a higher increase in delta band power compared to the sham stimulation (P=0.02, t=2.53). Moreover, the real stimulation caused a higher decrease in the power of beta band frequencies compared to the sham stimulation (at FP1: P=0.003, t=-3.75; at P4: P=0.01, t=-3.02). A similar trend was observed for the sub-bands of beta frequencies only at the prefrontal regions (at FP1 for Beta 1: P=0.02, t=-2.62; for beta 2: P=0.02, t=-2.74).



Moreover, a significant difference was also observed at the lower gamma band frequencies of prefrontal EEG activities (P=0.01, t=-2.77), t values are presented to compare the effect of real and sham stimulations. In addition, the association between changes in the power of brain activities and behavioral results is presented in Figure 2. The results showed that increased relative power of delta band activities at the prefrontal regions could increase the error responses to congruent color words. An increase in the relative power of the beta band activities at the prefrontal regions could increase the error responses to incongruent color words.




4. Discussion
Executive functions are an essential part of the brain’s cognitive performance. One of the main parameters of executive functioning is response inhibition. Malfunctions in response inhibition could cause errors in decision-making and planning. In this regard, studies have reported that ADHD individuals suffer from proper function of response inhibition. To improve the response inhibition in ADHD individuals, several studies have been conducted using pharmaceutical and other stimulation techniques.
It has been pointed out that stimulation of frontal areas, such as the DLPFC in ADHD individuals, can compensate for this deficit (Chen et al., 2021; Dubreuil-Vall et al., 2019). Among the NIBS paradigms, electrical and magnetic stimulations showed proper capacity for this purpose. Nevertheless, these methods require multiple intervention sessions to show their effectiveness on individuals with ADHD. It is supposed that improving TMS spatial resolution could enhance the effect of TMS while decreasing the number of intervention sessions. Since the micro-TMS has a higher spatial resolution than the TMS, we aimed to investigate the effectiveness of the single-session low-intensity magnetic stimulation method on the response inhibition of ADHD individuals. Therefore, the participants’ performances in SCWT before and after a real or sham stimulation were evaluated. Previous studies show that impairment in inhibition function is presented in the number of error responses to the SCWT. 
Consequently, DLPFC was selected as the target region for stimulation with micro-TMS, as described in the method section. The performance of ADHD subjects before and after the stimulation with micro-TMS was compared to quantify the effect of this method. 
The results of the behavioral data show that this stimulation protocol can improve response inhibition and decrease the error responses even after a single stimulation session. In addition, an analysis of the EEG data showed that significant changes in the relative power of the delta band at the frontal area were observed after the stimulation. The importance of brain activities in delta and theta frequencies at the frontal regions for proper functioning in response inhibition has been reported in previous studies as well (Ardolino et al., 2005; Clarke et al., 2002; Jacobson et al., 2012; Keeser et al., 2011). Furthermore, significant results of sham stimulation during incongruent trials would need to be further studied.
Previous studies have reported similar findings regarding the results observed in the beta frequency bands. For instance, studies have reported that beta and alpha band activity changes can affect subjects’ performance in response inhibition (Liao et al., 2021). Moreover, it has been shown that magnetic stimulation on DLPFC can improve response inhibition (Zrenner et al., 2020). Therefore, changes in the relative power of beta frequency activities after the stimulation with micro-TMS could indicate the proposed paradigm’s proper functioning. In addition, a significant correlation observed between error responses and the relative power of delta and beta bands at the prefrontal region could also be considered a proper marker for protocol effectiveness. 
In summary, the results of the present study show that the introduced stimulation protocol can improve response inhibition in ADHD people by changing the brain activities in the frontal regions. In addition, it seems that the introduced single-session stimulation protocol can improve response inhibition and may decrease the intervention sessions. Nevertheless, more investigation on more participants and examination of the method on other disorders considering co-factors such as gender and comorbidities are proposed for future studies.

Ethical Considerations

Compliance with ethical guidelines

All ethical principles are considered in this article. The participants were informed of the purpose of the research and its implementation stages. They were also assured about the confidentiality of their information and were free to leave the study whenever they wished, and if desired, the research results would be available to them. A written consent has been obtained from the subjects. Principles of the Helsinki Convention was also observed.

Funding
This research did not receive any grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or non-profit sectors.

Authors' contributions
All authors equally contributed to preparing this article.

Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments
The authors want to thank the Institute for Cognitive and Brain Sciences at Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran for their generous support in data recording.


References
Abrahamyan, A., Clifford, C. W., Arabzadeh, E., & Harris, J. A. (2015). Low-intensity TMS enhances perception of visual stimuli. Brain Stimulation, 8(6), 1175-1182. [DOI:10.1016/j.brs.2015.06.012] [PMID]
Acosta, M. T., & Leon-Sarmiento, F. E. (2003). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS): New tool, new therapy and new hope for ADHD. Current Medical Research and Opinion, 19(2), 125-130. [DOI:10.1080/030079903125001541] [PMID]
Acosta, M. T., Montañez, P., & Leon-Sarmiento, F. E. (2002). Half brain but not half function. Lancet (London, England), 360(9333), 643. [DOI:10.1016/S0140-6736(02)09793-3] [PMID]
Ardolino, G., Bossi, B., Barbieri, S., & Priori, A. (2005). Nonsynaptic mechanisms underlie the aftereffects of cathodal transcutaneous direct current stimulation of the human brain. The Journal of Physiology, 568(2), 653-663. [DOI:10.1113/jphysiol.2005.088310] [PMID]
Barbaresi, W. J., Katusic, S. K., Colligan, R. C., Pankratz, V. S., Weaver, A. L., & Weber, K. J., et al. (2002). How common is attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Incidence in a population-based birth cohort in Rochester, Minn. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 156(3), 217-224. [DOI:10.1001/archpedi.156.3.217] [PMID]
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65-94. [DOI:10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65] [PMID]
Barkley, R. A. (2000). Genetics of childhood disorders: XVII. ADHD, Part 1: The executive functions and ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(8), 1064-1068. [DOI:10.1097/00004583-200008000-00025] [PMID]
Blasi, G., Goldberg, T. E., Weickert, T., Das, S., Kohn, P., & Zoltick, B., et al. (2006). Brain regions underlying response inhibition and interference monitoring and suppression. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 23(6), 1658–1664. [DOI:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04680.x] [PMID]
Boggio, P. S., Rocha, M., Oliveira, M. O., Fecteau, S., Cohen, R. B., & Campanhã, C., et al. (2010). Noninvasive brain stimulation with high-frequency and low-intensity repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 71(8), 992–999. [DOI:10.4088/JCP.08m04638blu] [PMID]
Breitling, C., Zaehle, T., Dannhauer, M., Tegelbeckers, J., Flechtner, H. H., & Krauel, K. (2020). Comparison between conventional and HD-tDCS of the right inferior frontal gyrus in children and adolescents with ADHD. Clinical Neurophysiology, 131(5), 1146-1154. [DOI:10.1016/j.clinph.2019.12.412] [PMID]
Chen, T., Wang, H., Wang, X., Zhu, C., Zhang, L., Wang, K., & Yu, F. (2021). Transcranial direct current stimulation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex improves response inhibition. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 162, 34-39. [DOI:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.01.014] [PMID]
Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., Bond, D., McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz, M. (2002). Effects of stimulant medications on the EEG of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychopharmacology, 164(3), 277-284. [DOI:10.1007/s00213-002-1205-0] [PMID]
Colella, M., Laher, R., Press, D., McIlduff, C., Rutkove, S., Liberti, M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Bonmassar, G. (2019). A microTMS system for peripheral nerve stimulation. Brain Stimulation: Basic, Translational, and Clinical Research in Neuromodulation, 12(2), 521. [Link]
Cosmo, C., DiBiasi, M., Lima, V., Grecco, L. C., Muszkat, M., & Philip, N. S., et al. (2020). A systematic review of transcranial direct current stimulation effects in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 276, 1-13. [DOI:10.1016/j.jad.2020.06.054] [PMID]
Croarkin, P. E., Wall, C. A., & Lee, J. (2011). Applications of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in child and adolescent psychiatry. International Review of Psychiatry, 23(5), 445-453. [DOI:10.3109/09540261.2011.623688] [PMID]
Cuffe, S. P., McKeown, R. E., Jackson, K. L., Addy, C. L., Abramson, R., & Garrison, C. Z. (2001). Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a community sample of older adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(9), 1037-1044. [DOI:10.1097/00004583-200109000-00012] [PMID]
Dubreuil-Vall, L., Chau, P., Ruffini, G., Widge, A. S., & Camprodon, J. A. (2019). tDCS to the left DLPFC modulates cognitive and physiological correlates of executive function in a state-dependent manner. Brain Stimulation, 12(6), 1456-1463. [DOI:10.1016/j.brs.2019.06.006] [PMID]
Dubreuil-Vall, L., Gomez-Bernal, F., Villegas, A. C., Cirillo, P., Surman, C., Ruffini, G., Widge, A. S., & Camprodon, J. A. (2020). tDCS to the left DLPFC improves cognitive control but not action cancellation in patients with ADHD: A behavioral and electrophysiological study. medRxiv. [DOI:10.1101/2020.01.13.20017335]
DuPaul, G. J., Guevremont, D. C., & Barkley, R. A. (1991). Attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder in adolescence: Critical assessment parameters. Clinical Psychology Review, 11(3), 231-245. [DOI:10.1016/0272-7358(91)90102-Z]
Fischer, M., Barkley, R. A., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2005). Executive functioning in hyperactive children as young adults: attention, inhibition, response perseveration, and the impact of comorbidity. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(1), 107-133. [DOI:10.1207/s15326942dn2701_5] [PMID]
Friehs, M. A., Frings, C., & Hartwigsen, G. (2021). Effects of single-session transcranial direct current stimulation on reactive response inhibition. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 128, 749-765. [DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.07.013] [PMID]
Garavan, H., Ross, T., & Stein, E. (1999). Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: An event-related functional MRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96(14), 8301-8306. [DOI:10.1073/pnas.96.14.8301] [PMID]
Gorfein, D. S., & MacLeod, C. M. (2007). Inhibition in cognition. American Psychological Association. [DOI:10.1037/11587-000]
Grosbras, M.-H., & Paus, T. (2002). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field: Effects on visual perception and attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(7), 1109-1120. [DOI:10.1162/089892902320474553] [PMID]
Hallett, M. (2001). Functional reorganization after lesions of the human brain: studies with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Revue Neurologique, 157(8-9 Pt 1), 822-826. [PMID]
Harnishfeger, K. K., & Bjorklund, D. F. (1993). The ontogeny of inhibition mechanisms: A renewed approach to cognitive development. In: Howe ML, Pasnak R, editors. Emerging themes in cognitive development. New York: Springer. [DOI:10.1007/978-1-4613-9220-0_2]
Heinen, K., Feredoes, E., Weiskopf, N., Ruff, C. C., & Driver, J. (2014). Direct evidence for attention-dependent influences of the frontal eye-fields on feature-responsive visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 24(11), 2815-2821. [DOI:10.1093/cercor/bht157] [PMID]
Jacobson, L., Ezra, A., Berger, U., & Lavidor, M. (2012). Modulating oscillatory brain activity correlates of behavioral inhibition using transcranial direct current stimulation. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(5), 979-984. [DOI:10.1016/j.clinph.2011.09.016] [PMID]
Jiang, X., Liu, L., Ji, H., & Zhu, Y. (2018). Association of affected neurocircuitry with deficit of response inhibition and delayed gratification in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A narrative review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 506. [DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2018.00506] [PMID]
Keeser, D., Padberg, F., Reisinger, E., Pogarell, O., Kirsch, V., & Palm, U., et al. (2011). Prefrontal direct current stimulation modulates resting EEG and event-related potentials in healthy subjects: A standardized low resolution tomography (sLORETA) study. Neuroimage, 55(2), 644-657. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.004] [PMID]
Khodadadi, M., Feyzi Daryati, M. R., Movahedi, Y., & Ahmadi, I. (2014). [Assessment of attention bias in the cognitive processing of neutral and emotional words using semantic strop test (Persian)]. Shenakht Journal of Psychology & Psychiatry, 1(1), 23-30. [Link]
King, J. A., Colla, M., Brass, M., Heuser, I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2007). Inefficient cognitive control in adult ADHD: Evidence from trial-by-trial Stroop test and cued task switching performance. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3, 42. [PMID]
Liao, Y. C., Guo, N. W., Su, B. Y., Chen, S. J., Tsai, H. F., & Lee, K. Y. (2021). Frontal Beta activity in the meta-intention of children With attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 52(2), 136-143. [DOI:10.1177/1550059420933142] [PMID]
Neuman, R. J., Sitdhiraksa, N., Reich, W., Ji, T. H., Joyner, C. A., & Sun, L. W., et al. (2005). Estimation of prevalence of DSM-IV and latent class-defined ADHD subtypes in a population-based sample of child and adolescent twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 8(4), 392-401. [PMID]
Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 220-246. [DOI:10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.220] [PMID]
Nigg, J. T. (2001). Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? Psychological Bulletin, 127(5), 571-598. [DOI:10.1037//0033-2909.127.5.571] [PMID]
Ridding, M. C., & Rothwell, J. C. (2007). Is there a future for therapeutic use of transcranial magnetic stimulation? Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 8(7), 559–567. [DOI:10.1038/nrn2169] [PMID]
Song, Y., & Hakoda, Y. (2011). An asymmetric stroop/reverse-stroop interference phenomenon in ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 15(6), 499-505. [DOI:10.1177/1087054710367607] [PMID]
Tamm, L., Menon, V., & Reiss, A. L. (2002). Maturation of brain function associated with response inhibition. Journal of The American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(10), 1231-1238. [DOI:10.1097/00004583-200210000-00013] [PMID]
Tang, A. D., Bennett, W., Hadrill, C., Collins, J., Fulopova, B., & Wills, K., et al. (2018). Low-intensity repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation modulates skilled motor learning in adult mice. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 4016. [DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-22385-8] [PMID]
Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1336-1346. [DOI:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006] [PMID]
Zaman, R. (2015). Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Psychiatria Danubina, 27(suppl 1), 530-532. [PMID]
Zrenner, B., Zrenner, C., Gordon, P. C., Belardinelli, P., McDermott, E. J., & Soekadar, S. R., et al. (2020). Brain oscillation-synchronized stimulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in depression using real-time EEG-triggered TMS. Brain Stimulation, 13(1), 197-205. [DOI:10.1016/j.brs.2019.10.007] [PMID]
Type of Study: Original | Subject: Cognitive Neuroscience
Received: 2022/10/9 | Accepted: 2022/11/14 | Published: 2024/07/20

Add your comments about this article : Your username or Email:
CAPTCHA

Send email to the article author


Rights and permissions
Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

© 2024 CC BY-NC 4.0 | Basic and Clinical Neuroscience

Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb