1. Introduction
Mirror neurons are the neurons that fire during the observation and execution of actions. The mirror neuron system (MNS) was first observed in the frontal cortex of macaque monkeys (Di Pellegrino et al., 1992). Later, human MNS was discovered in the motor cortex, which occupies the posterior precentral gyrus in the frontal area (Fadiga et al., 1995; Kilner et al., 2009). Although MNS has been described initially in imitating actions (Jeannerod & Decety, 1995), further research illustrates that it is also essential for perceiving others’ mental states (Gallese & Goldman, 1998; Luyten & Fonagy, 2015). This finding suggests that a fundamental process allowing us to appreciate the actions and emotions of others involves the activation of the MNS (Fonagy & Luyten, 2016). MNS is also valence-sensitive; the valence of an emotional presentation (positive or negative) can influence subsequent mirror neuron activity (Enticott et al., 2012), so it has higher excitability to negative emotional stimuli (Schmidt et al., 2020). Therefore, MNS, as a fundamental base for understanding others, facilitates “social cognition” in a healthy brain. Impaired MNS has been described in the pathophysiology of several psychiatric conditions, such as autism spectrum disorder (Hadjikhani et al., 2006), schizophrenia (Mehta et al., 2014), depression (Nejati, 2018; Nejati et al., 2012), and psychopath (Fecteau et al., 2008).
Social cognition is the ability to process social stimuli characterized by a variety of interpersonal skills such as self-perception (Nejati et al., 2012), the theory of mind (Mier et al., 2009), empathy (Corradini & Antonietti, 2013), intention understanding (Catmur, 2015), facial (Enticott et al., 2008) and emotion recognition (Nejati et al., 2022). These skills are gathered under an umbrella concept called mentalization capacity (Luyten & Fonagy, 2015). As a form of social cognition, mentalization enables us to perceive and interpret human behavior through intentional mental states, concluding needs, desires, feelings, beliefs, and goals (Fonagy & Luyten, 2009). The acquisition of this capacity depends on the quality of early bonding with the mother or other attachment figures (Fonagy, 2011). Therefore, the child’s early attachment is the primary foundation for developing social cognition (Ziv & Arbel, 2020). Furthermore, mentalization is likely to vary greatly among specific relationships instead of general ones. General circumstances in this setting represent individuals’ general emotions, whereas relational conditions raise mental representations specific to each individual’s primary attachments (Fonagy & Luyten, 2009; Overall et al., 2003). Although overlapping, general and relational mentalization appears distinct (Happe & Frith, 1996; Humfress et al., 2002; O’Connor & Hirsch, 1999). It has been observed that when children (Humfress et al., 2002) or adolescents (O’Connor & Hirsch, 1999) are exposed to situations triggering general emotions vs relational emotions, they show different mentalizing capacity. Children with avoidant attachment styles had greater difficulty mentalizing relational stimuli than general stimuli due to their lower mentalization capacity. However, this difficulty was less in the secure attachment group. Based on these findings, it can be assumed that mentalizing relational stimuli requires a higher level of mentalization (Humfress et al., 2002; O’Connor & Hirsch, 1999; Repacholi & Trapolini, 2004). Accordingly, the within-person variation in the capacity to mentalize others supports this capacity, which contains both general and relational representations (Fonagy & Luyten, 2009; Overall et al., 2003). This fact raises the possibility that relational content requires a higher mentalization capacity than general emotional content. To put it another way, there is an assumption that MNS activity might be manipulated differently by relational vs general emotions triggered by social situations.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), a non-invasive brain stimulation technique, is a well-documented tool for studying the MNS. TMS applies a brief magnetic pulse to the underlying brain structure through surface scalp coils (Terao & Ugawa, 2002). In social cognition studies, the peripheral muscle activity, measured by electromyography after applying TMS pulses over the primary motor cortex (PMC), has been described as an indicator of the MNS function (Maeda et al., 2002). Altogether, both action observation and facing situations that require understanding other’s mental states activate mirror neurons, thus increasing the PMC excitability, which results in an enhanced motor evoked potential (MEP) amplitude (Enticott et al., 2008; Maeda et al., 2002).
The present study investigated the MNS responses to relational vs general content. We hypothesize that PMS excitability will be different in these two distinct conditions. To do so, we delivered TMS pulses during both active and static hand observations. At the same time, participants were exposed to images with relational vs general stimuli of both negative and positive valence. Based on previous findings (Enticott et al., 2012; Hill et al., 2013), it was hypothesized that exposure to negative compared to positive stimuli enhances MNS activity in the general context. Also, there might be a difference between MNS activity when presenting relational stimuli.
2. Materials and Methods
Study participants
The sample consisted of 28 female adults (age: Mean±SD 39.4±13.1, range=21-61 years) selected by voluntary response sampling method. The inclusion criteria include right-handedness, as assessed by the Edinburgh-Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971), an age range of 18 to 40 years, and no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders. As previous studies stimulated each cortical hemisphere separately and found larger MEP amplitude in negative and positive trials in the left hemisphere (Aziz‐Zadeh et al., 2004; Hill et al., 2013), we decided only to stimulate the left M1 and, therefore, excluded left-handed participants.
All stages of the research were carried out between October and March 2021-2022. Before participating, the candidates were screened concerning TMS safety criteria (Rossi et al., 2009). The Ethics Committees of Iran University of Medical Sciences granted ethical clearance for the project, which was carried out in the Nasional Brain Mapping Lab (NBML). Participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Each participant gave written informed consent, and at the end, they were honored with cash and non-cash gifts.
Study materials
Visual stimuli
Participants are shown five blocks: a) General emotion, including negative and positive; b) Relational emotion, including negative and positive; and c) Neutral stimuli. Each block contains:
I. Videos are 3-second long and feature either 1- a right hand performing a transitive movement (picking up a mug, Figure 1) or 2- a static right hand next to a mug. Consistent with previous research, the transitive hand movements are used to elicit an MNS response, whereas the static hands are employed as a control condition and have been previously shown not to activate the MNS (Enticott et al., 2012; Enticott et al., 2008)
II. Images
1. Images for general emotion (positive and negative) and neutral blocks were taken from the international affective picture system (IAPS). The IAPS contains a large bank of emotionally evocative images, which have been rated for emotional valence and arousal by a large American cohort (Lang, 2005). These images were selected based on their valence and arousal properties as rated by this cohort. As IAPS images are rated differently by males and females, we only selected from the images related to the females’.
2- Images for the relational emotion (positive and negative) blocks were taken from the attachment-related picture set (ARPS). The ARPS contains a bank of emotional attachment and pictures rated by 310 individuals for valence and arousal (Maleki et al., 2021).
We had 5 emotional blocks of 40 trials each. Each trial contains an image displayed for 3 s, followed by the video clip (3 s) of the static or transitive hand in a quasi-random order, followed by 1 s of black screen (a total of 7s: Figure 1).
Study procedure
Participants were comfortably seated on a recliner chair 60 cm from a 22-LCD monitor. EMG electrodes are placed over the first dorsal interosseous (FDI), as mug grasping involves activating the right hand’s FDI muscle and abductor digiti minimi (ADM) muscles as a control site. To ensure low skin impedance, the electrode sites were cleaned with alcohol. The EMG signal was amplified using an MA300 system (Motion Lab System, CO) with low/high pass filtering set at 500 Hz and 10 Hz, respectively.
A single TMS pulse at 100% resting motor threshold (RMT) was delivered over the left M1 through a 70 mm figure-of-eight coil powered by a MagPro X100 stimulator (Magventure company, USA). When stimulated, the M1 area was defined as the site that produced the largest MEP in the FDI muscle. The RMT was defined as the lowest stimulus intensity required to produce a reliable peak-to-peak MEP amplitude of approximately 1 mv in the FDI muscle (Mean±SD RMT=57±9.7). The decision to stimulate the left hemispheres was made based on the participants’ handedness.
During the video clip presentations, a jittered TMS pulse occurred in the video frame right before the hand grasps the mug (corticospinal excitability [CSE] is maximal immediately before an object is grasped with the hand) (Gangitano et al., 2001), and, for the static hand video, TMS pulses occurred the same second into the video clip.
Statistical analysis
The peak-to-peak MEP amplitude was calculated with Matlab software (R2019b, MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA). To correct muscle contraction-contaminate EMG activity, we mean substitute trials in which an EMG activity within 200 ms of the TMS pulses was evident (0.1% of all trials). Moreover, an outlier removal using a performance package (Lüdecke et al., 2021) was conducted to remove the effect of influential trials (involuntary movements, sensor drift, etc.). As previously suggested, to control the inflation of the MEP responses (Enticott et al., 2012), median peak-to-peak amplitudes for each of the static and transitive hand videos were employed to compute the MEP log ratio (MEP-LR). MEP-LR was calculated using the Equation 1:
1. MEP-LR=10(log MEPtransitive/MEPstatic
This estimation provides a relative index of putative mirror neuron activity, in which larger and positive MEP-LRs values show greater MNS responses.
All statistical analyses were carried out in the R (version 4.1.2) environment (Team, 2013) using afex (Singmann et al., 2015) and ggstatsplot (Patil, 2021) packages. The normality and homogeneity of variance were assured by the Shapiro-Wilk and Levene’s tests, respectively. Repeated measures ANOVA were conducted for the dependent variable (MEP-LR), with Image types (relational positive, general positive, relational negative, general negative, nature) and muscle (FDI, ADM) as the within-subject factors. Mauchly’s test of sphericity was conducted, and the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied when necessary. Post hoc analyses were calculated using false discovery rate (FDR)-corrected pairwise student t-tests (two-tailed).
3. Results
MNS activity and muscle specificity
No significant image types × muscle interaction was found (F(4, 108)=0.26, P=0.90, ηp2=0.01; 95% CI, 0.00%, 1.00%), indicating that MEP-LR values in response to the stimuli were not muscle specific. However, subsequent FDR-corrected paired sample t-test revealed a significant difference between positive and negative emotional stimuli in general image type in FDI muscle (t27=3.12, P=0.04, dcohen=0.59) (Figure 1).
No main effect of Muscle was found (F(1, 27)=0.93; P=0.34, ηp2=0.03; 95% CI, 0.00%, 1.00%). However, as was expected, the overall MEP-LR value was higher in the FDI muscle compared to ADM (Mean±SE FDI: 0.35±0.12, ADM: 0.24±0.13) (Figure 2).
MNS activity and emotional valence
A significant medium main effect of image types was observed (F(4, 108)=3.65; P=0.008, ηp2=0.119; 95%CI, 0.02%, 1.00%). Given that, no muscle-specific effect on image types was found. We performed an FDR-corrected paired sample t-test to investigate the overall impact of image types across collapsed FDI and ADM muscles. The Post hoc comparisons revealed a significant effect between neutral and negative emotion in general image type (t27=2.81, P=0.02, dcohen=0.38). In addition, a significant difference between positive and negative emotional stimuli was observed both in relational (t27=2.48, P=0.03, dcohen=0.33) and general (t27=3.75, P=0.004, dcohen=0.50) image types. Furthermore, a difference between relational and general image types was also evident in positive (t27=2.79, P=0.02, dcohen=0.37) and negative (t27=2.99, P=0.02, dcohen=0.40) emotions (Figure 3).
4. Discussion
This study set out to test the modulation of emotion processing valence and relationality on MNS activity. This approach was achieved via a TMS paradigm during action observation relative to the observation of static control, indicating putative MNS activity (Enticott et al., 2012; Hill et al., 2013; Strafella & Paus, 2000). Concerning the first hypothesis, which addresses the effect of general emotion processing on MNS activity, we found an increased MEP amplitude in FDI muscle (but not in ADM muscle) following positive compared to negative images in general conditions. In addition, we observed increased MEP amplitude in neutral relative to general negative images across collapsed FDI and ADM muscles. Interestingly, regarding the second hypothesis considering the effect of relational emotion on MNS activity, this study revealed a difference between positive and negative emotions in both general and relational conditions. However, this finding was only significant across collapsed FDI and ADM muscles, and no muscle specificity was observed. Therefore, we conclude that (1) General positive vs general negative emotion, (2) Relational negative vs relational positive, (3) Relational negative vs general negative, and (4) General positive vs relational positive modulate MNS activity. Our finding supports the evidence for the modulatory effect of emotions with different valences on the MNS activity (Enticott et al., 2012; Hill et al., 2013) by showing the impact of processing emotions with different valences on mirror neuron MEP amplitude. However, unlike Enticott et al. (2012), who observed an increased MNS activity following the negative images and not the positive ones, we replicated Hill et al. (2013). We observed this increased activity following positive versus negative images in the general condition in FDI muscle.
Furthermore, in the current study for the first time, in addition to valence, we also investigated the effect of relational emotion processing on MNS function. Surprisingly, our results showed that MNS activity across positive and negative emotions was altered under both relational and general conditions. This finding supports the assumption that relational content interferes with mentalizing capacity.
MNS activity and emotional valence
Compared to increased MNS activity following exposure to negative stimuli, it is less clear why positive emotions might induce an augmented MNS response. There are more assumptions about the involvement of negative stimuli in MNS activity, including the likelihood of their occurrence in situations that threaten one’s survival, which associate them with fast and decisive actions to help protect an individual from physical harm (Fredrickson, 2001). However, taking the social cognitive perspective into account, positive interactions with others could increase MNS function as well, which, in turn, would enhance social cognitive abilities such as emotional engagement and empathic understanding (Gallese & Goldman, 1998). The advantages of improved social cognitive skills can be seen in positive situations, for example, when one can engage more strongly with the environment and other people (Fredrickson, 2001). Therefore, the current result that shows general positive emotion exerts a more facilitatory effect on MNS than general negative emotion can be argued for from a social cognitive perspective. However, besides the difference between positive and negative emotions, we observed an increased MEP amplitude following neutral compared to negative emotions. The facilitatory effect of neutral emotion on MNS activity was unexpected. However, considering the cultural differences, previous studies show that continuous exposure to violent content in the media eventually causes desensitization (Tarabah et al., 2016). Images being rated as negative in the IAPS bank are evaluated from an American perspective, while they are often seen in the Iranian culture and cause a kind of desensitization in the audience (for example images related to war and traumas such as torture). In addition, the American people’s images rated as neutral in IAPS bank might not be completely neutral in our participants’ culture. The most promising example is the image of an airplane, which is generally a neutral image. In Iranian culture, it might, however, remind people of the tragic experience of Ukraine international airlines flight 752. Undoubtedly, this issue needs further investigation.
MNS activity and relational emotion
The novelty of our finding is consistent with data showing that relational emotion influences mentalization capacity. It has been observed that when children (Humfress et al., 2002) or adolescents (O’Connor & Hirsch, 1999) are exposed to situations triggering general emotions relative to relational emotions, they show different mentalizing capacities. More specifically, as reported by Repacholi and Trapolini (2004), children with high scores on the avoidance dimension of the separation anxiety test show less mentalizing capacity in the case of a mother-child relationship. Humfress et al. (2002) consistently reported that children who exhibited a less coherent attachment model were more likely to be rated as exhibiting a dismissing/avoidant style in the attachment interview. Accordingly, attachment representations characterized by high levels of avoidance appear to interfere with children’s ability to fully engage their social-cognitive skills when reasoning about maternal mental states (Repacholi & Trapolini, 2004). These findings align with studies suggesting the involvement of MNS in understanding others’ mental states (Gallese & Goldman, 1998; Schmidt et al., 2020) and raise the possibility that relational content requires a higher mentalization capacity than general emotional content. Fonagy and Loyten (2009) took it one step further and discussed it in the context of more insecure patients, e.g. individuals diagnosed with a borderline personality disorder. They claimed that the stronger the attachment in a particular relationship at a specific moment, the more likely that anomalies in mentalization will emerge in bipolar personality disorder patients. Our TMS-EMG result confirms this assumption, which adds to Repacholi and Trapolini’s (2004) findings and reveals an altered MNS activity following relational emotion relative to general emotion. They observed that relational stimuli were more difficult for avoidant children. Because they demanded more mentalization capacity, they observed that relational stimuli were more difficult for avoidant children. As a result, it demanded more mentalization capacity, and these children could not meet that demand due to their avoidant attachment style. However, our result reveal that the processing of relational negative emotion compared to relational positive one and relational negative compared to the general negative one exerts an upregulation effect on the MNS function of our participants. This outcome could be because mentalizing relational stimuli requires a higher capacity for mentalization, which can be shown by more MNS activity in a relational negative situation. However, the augmented MNS activity following general positive relative to relational positive images could be explained by the circle of security model (Fonagy et al., 2018; Maxwell et al., 2021), arguing that in the first place, children need more mentalizing capacity for processing negative emotions, which enable them to process negative encounters (during coordination with their mother). In parallel, experiencing positive emotions would pave their way towards tolerating some distance from the mother and socializing. We have also seen that images related to primary negative relationships, require more mentalizing capacity, indicating more MNS activity. On the other hand, experiencing positive emotion in a general condition, which triggers the second half of the circle of the security model, initiates the socialization process and therefore increases the MNS activity as well.
In short, an individual needs a higher mentalization capacity during relational negative emotions (initial emotions) and when experiencing general positive emotions (not necessarily relational ones).
MNS activity and muscle specificity
The result shows a clear pattern of muscle specificity of the MNS response following positive relative to negative images in general conditions but not in the ADM muscle. This finding aligns with a previous TMS study on mirror neurons (Enticott et al., 2012). In the current study, we employed a hand action designed to primarily activate the FDI (picking up a cup by the handle), and participants’ recorded MEP-log values for the FDI muscle were larger than those for the ADM when participants viewed transitive hand movement. However, this muscle specificity has not been found in relational trials.
5. Conclusion
The current study supports the literature on the modulatory effect of emotions with different valences on MNS activity by reporting the augmented mirror neuron’s MEP amplitude following the processing of general positive emotions compared to general negative emotions. Additionally, for the first time, we investigated the effect of relational emotion processing on MNS function. We observed an altered MNS activity following relational compared to general stimuli, which supports the assumption that relation-based content interferes with mentalizing capacity.
Study limitations and future direction
Some limitations should be taken into account in the present study. First, this study is exploratory, with a relatively limited number of healthy participants and some caution in clinical application. Furthermore, given the valence theory of emotion, the right and left hemispheres are involved in negative and positive emotional processing, respectively and it would be compared to both hemispheres in the study.
In addition, using a condition including the mug without the presence of the hand helps to control “the expectation effect” in the subject, which was not considered in this research due to the large number of trials.
Ethical Considerations
Compliance with ethical guidelines
The Ethics Committees of Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran, granted ethical clearance for the project.
Funding
This research was partially funded by the Cognitive Sciences and Technologies Council, Tehran, Iran. The funder was not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication.
Authors' contributions
All authors contributed equally to the conception and design of the study, data collection and analysis, interception of the results and drafting of the manuscript. Each author approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.
Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank the Iranian National Brain Mapping Laboratory (NBML), Tehran, Iran, for providing data acquisition and analysis services for this research work. The authors are also grateful to the Iran High-Tech Laboratory Network for financial support.
References
Aziz-Zadeh, L., Iacoboni, M., Zaidel, E., Wilson, S., & Mazziotta, J. (2004). Left hemisphere motor facilitation in response to manual action sounds. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 19(9), 2609–2612. [DOI:10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03348.x] [PMID]
Catmur, C. (2015). Understanding intentions from actions: Direct perception, inference, and the roles of mirror and mentalizing systems. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 426–433.[DOI:10.1016/j.concog.2015.03.012] [PMID]
Corradini, A., & Antonietti, A. (2013). Mirror neurons and their function in cognitively understood empathy. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(3), 1152–1161. [DOI:10.1016/j.concog.2013.03.003] [PMID]
Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91(1), 176-180. [DOI:10.1007/BF00230027] [PMID]
Enticott, P. G., Harrison, B. A., Arnold, S. L., Nibaldi, K., Segrave, R. A., & Fitzgibbon, B. M., et al. (2012). Emotional valence modulates putative mirror neuron activity. Neuroscience Letters, 508(1), 56–59. [DOI:10.1016/j.neulet.2011.12.018] [PMID]
Enticott, P. G., Johnston, P. J., Herring, S. E., Hoy, K. E., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2008). Mirror neuron activation is associated with facial emotion processing. Neuropsychologia, 46(11), 2851–2854. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.04.022] [PMID]
Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facilitation during action observation: A magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73(6), 2608-2611. [DOI:10.1152/jn.1995.73.6.2608] [PMID]
Fecteau, S., Pascual-Leone, A., & Théoret, H. (2008). Psychopathy and the mirror neuron system: Preliminary findings from a non-psychiatric sample. Psychiatry Research, 160(2), 137-144. [DOI:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.08.022] [PMID]
Fonagy, P. (2011). The mentalization-focused approach to social development. In F. N. Busch (Ed.), Mentalization (pp. 21-74). New York: Routledge. [DOI:10.4324/9780203927250-9]
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., & Jurist, E. L. (2018). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. London: Routledge. [DOI:10.4324/9780429471643]
Fonagy, P., & Luyten, P. (2009). A developmental, mentalization-based approach to the understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 21(4), 1355-1381. [DOI:10.1017/S0954579409990198] [PMID]
Fonagy, P., & Luyten, P. (2016). A multilevel perspective on the development of borderline personality disorder. Developmental Psychopathology, 1-67. [DOI:10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy317]
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. The American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226. [DOI:10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218] [PMID]
Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(12), 493-501. [DOI:10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01262-5] [PMID]
Gangitano, M., Mottaghy, F. M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Phase-specific modulation of cortical motor output during movement observation. Neuroreport, 12(7), 1489-1492. [DOI:10.1097/00001756-200105250-00038] [PMID]
Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. M., Snyder, J., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2006). Anatomical differences in the mirror neuron system and social cognition network in autism. Cerebral Cortex, 16(9), 1276-1282. [DOI:10.1093/cercor/bhj069] [PMID]
Happe, F., & Frith, U. (1996). The neuropsychology of autism. Brain, 119(4), 1377-1400. [DOI:10.1093/brain/119.4.1377] [PMID]
Hill, A. T., Fitzgibbon, B. M., Arnold, S. L., Rinehart, N. J., Fitzgerald, P. B., & Enticott, P. G. (2013). Modulation of putative mirror neuron activity by both positively and negatively valenced affective stimuli: A TMS study. Behavioural Brain Research, 249, 116-123. [DOI:10.1016/j.bbr.2013.04.027] [PMID]
Humfress, H., O’Connor, T. G., Slaughter, J., Target, M., & Fonagy, P. (2002). General and relationship‐specific models of social cognition: Explaining the overlap and discrepancies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(7), 873-883. [DOI:10.1111/1469-7610.0013_7] [PMID]
Jeannerod, M., & Decety, J. (1995). Mental motor imagery: A window into the representational stages of action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5(6), 727-732. [DOI:10.1016/0959-4388(95)80099-9] [PMID]
Kilner, J. M., Neal, A., Weiskopf, N., Friston, K. J., & Frith, C. D. (2009). Evidence of mirror neurons in human inferior frontal gyrus. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(32), 10153–10159. [DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2668-09.2009] [PMID]
Lang, P. J. (2005). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report. [Link]
Lüdecke, D., Ben-Shachar, M. S., Patil, I., Waggoner, P., & Makowski, D. (2021). performance: An R package for assessment, comparison and testing of statistical models. Journal of Open Source Software, 6(60), 3139. [Link]
Luyten, P., & Fonagy, P. (2015). The neurobiology of mentalizing. Personality Disorders, 6(4), 366–379. [DOI:10.1037/per0000117] [PMID]
Maeda, F., Kleiner-Fisman, G., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2002). Motor facilitation while observing hand actions: Specificity of the effect and role of observer’s orientation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 87(3), 1329–1335. [DOI:10.1152/jn.00773.2000] [PMID]
Maleki, G., Mazaheri, M. A., Nejati, V., Borhani, K., & Bosmans, G. (2021). The Attachment-related picture set (ARPS): Development and validation. Current Psychology, 42, 3668–3679. [DOI:10.1007/s12144-021-01727-2]
Maxwell, A. M., Reay, R. E., Huber, A., Hawkins, E., Woolnough, E., & McMahon, C. (2021). Parent and practitioner perspectives on Circle of Security Parenting (COS‐P): A qualitative study. Infant Mental Health Journal, 42(3), 452-468. [DOI:10.1002/imhj.21916] [PMID]
Mehta, U. M., Thirthalli, J., Aneelraj, D., Jadhav, P., Gangadhar, B. N., & Keshavan, M. S. (2014). Mirror neuron dysfunction in schizophrenia and its functional implications: A systematic review. Schizophrenia Research, 160(1-3), 9-19. [DOI:10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.040] [PMID]
Mier, D., Zentgraf, K., Reiser, M., Zygrodnik, K., Munzert, J., & Gallhofer, et al. (2009). The role of the mirror neuron network for theory of mind: An fMRI study using point-light representations of biological motion. NeuroImage, 47(Supplement 1), S184. [DOI:10.1016/S1053-8119(09)72031-2]
Nejati, V. (2018). Negative interpretation of social cue in depression: Evidence from the reading mind from eyes test. Neurology, Psychiatry and Brain Research, 27, 12-16. [DOI:10.1016/j.npbr.2017.11.001]
Nejati, V., Khorrami, A. S., & Fonoudi, M. (2022). Neuromodulation of facial emotion recognition in health and disease: A systematic review. Neurophysiologie Clinique, 52(3), 183-201. [DOI:10.1016/j.neucli.2022.03.005]
Nejati, V., Zabihzadeh, A., Maleki, G., & Tehranchi, A. (2012). Mind reading and mindfulness deficits in patients with major depressive disorder. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 32, 431-437. [DOI:10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.01.065]
Nejati, V., Zabihzadeh, A., Nikfarjam, M. R., Pournaghdali, A., Naderi, Z., & Riyahi, M. T. (2012). Self as a social cue: Evidence for the reading mind from eyes test. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 32, 82-88. [DOI:10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.01.015]
O’Connor, T. G., & Hirsch, N. (1999). Intra‐individual differences and relationship‐specificity of mentalising in early adolescence. Social Development, 8(2), 256-274. [DOI:10.1111/1467-9507.00094]
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97-113. [DOI:10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4] [PMID]
Overall, N. C., Fletcher, G. J., & Friesen, M. D. (2003). Mapping the intimate relationship mind: Comparisons between three models of attachment representations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(12), 1479-1493. [DOI:10.1177/0146167203251519] [PMID]
Patil, I. (2021). Visualizations with statistical details: The ‘ggstatsplot’ approach. Journal of Open Source Software, 6(61), 3167. [DOI:10.21105/joss.03167]
Repacholi, B., & Trapolini, T. (2004). Attachment and preschool children’s understanding of maternal versus non‐maternal psychological states. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22(3), 395-415. [DOI:10.1348/0261510041552693]
Rossi, S., Hallett, M., Rossini, P. M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Safety of TMS Consensus Group (2009). Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120(12), 2008-2039. [DOI:10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016] [PMID]
Schmidt, S. N. L., Sojer, C. A., Hass, J., Kirsch, P., & Mier, D. (2020). fMRI adaptation reveals: The human mirror neuron system discriminates emotional valence. Cortex, 128, 270-280. [DOI:10.1016/j.cortex.2020.03.026] [PMID]
Singmann, H. (2015). Afex: Analysis of factorial experiments (Version 0.13-145). Retrieved from: [Link]
Strafella, A. P., & Paus, T. (2000). Modulation of cortical excitability during action observation: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuroreport, 11(10), 2289-2292. [DOI:10.1097/00001756-200007140-00044] [PMID]
Tarabah, A., Badr, L. K., Usta, J., & Doyle, J. (2016). Exposure to violence and children’s desensitization attitudes in Lebanon. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 31(18), 3017-3038. [DOI:10.1177/0886260515584337] [PMID]
Team, R. C. (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Retrieved from: [Link]
Terao, Y., & Ugawa, Y. (2002). Basic mechanisms of TMS. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology: Official Publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society, 19(4), 322–343.[DOI:10.1097/00004691-200208000-00006] [PMID]
Ziv, Y., & Arbel, R. (2020). Association between the mother’s social cognition and the child’s social functioning in kindergarten: The mediating role of the child’s social cognition. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(1), 358. [DOI:10.3390/ijerph17010358] [PMID]