Volume 13, Issue 4 (July & August: Special Issue on Cognitive Sciences 2022)                   BCN 2022, 13(4): 573-584 | Back to browse issues page


XML Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Narmashiri A, Hatami J, Khosrowabadi R, Sohrabi A. Resting-State Electroencephalogram (EEG) Coherence Over Frontal Regions in Paranormal Beliefs. BCN 2022; 13 (4) :573-584
URL: http://bcn.iums.ac.ir/article-1-2051-en.html
1- Institute of Cognitive Sciences Studies, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran.
2- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology & Education, University of Kurdistan, Sanandaj, Iran.
Full-Text [PDF 787 kb]       |   Abstract (HTML) 
Full-Text:  
1. Introduction
Paranormal belief is described as a belief in psychological, biological, or physical phenomena assuming the existence of core/basic ontological characteristics of other ontological categories. Thus, physical and biological phenomena may have psychological core characteristics (such as intentionality, desires, and beliefs) (Betsch, Jäckel, Hammes, & Brinkmann, 2021). It is also likely that physical and mental phenomena have the main characteristics of biological organisms (such as contamination, healing, and living). Also, mental phenomena can have the main characteristics of physical phenomenon, such as independent presence and energy (or force) and the ability to touch objects (Betsch, Aßmann, & Glöckner, 2020; Drinkwater, Denovan, Dagnall, & Parker, 2020; Lindeman & Aarnio, 2007; Lindeman & Saher, 2007; Wellman & Gelman, 1998). Paranormal beliefs are beliefs, entities, practices, and processes that contradict the basic limiting principles of science (FioRito, Abeyta, & Routledge., 2020) and include belief in traditional religion, extrasensory perception, witchcraft, superstition, telekinesis, spiritualism, magical thinking, and precognition (Wilson, 2018). Cognitive neuroscience research on paranormal ideas has proposed the paranormal belief executive inhibition hypothesis (Narmashiri, Hatami, Khosrowabadi, & Sohrabi, 2017, 2019, 2021; Cristofori et al., 2016; Wain & Spinella, 2007). 
As a reaction to authoritative propositions, executive down-regulation is theorized to underpin paranormal beliefs (Deeley, 2003). Since structural networks in frontal brain areas support executive functions, neuropsychological research has focused on the hypothesis of frontal executive inhibition, particularly dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) (Newberg, Wintering, Morgan, & Waldman, 2006). A theory was proposed for reducing cognitive resources in the monitoring of errors in religious rituals in the form of a paranormal phenomenon by Schjoedt and Sørensen (Schjoedt et al., 2013). Supporting the executive inhibition hypothesis, an activity reduction in dlPFC was found within paranormal experiences (Newberg et al., 2006). Participants were recently found to reduce dlPFC regions as a reaction to a charismatic speaker’s prayers (Schjoedt et al., 2011). Also, dlPFC was demonstrated to be substantially more activated among skeptics than among paranormal believers (Lindeman et al., 2013). 
Nonetheless, a large cortical network with dorsal cingulate, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex, and dlPFC was demonstrated to support cognitive control (Aron et al., 2004; Corbetta & Shulman, 2002). Research has viewed the dlPFC as a key brain structure for inhibition imposition, specifically concerning paranormal beliefs (Schjoedt, Stødkilde-Jørgensen, Geertz, Lund, & Roepstorff, 2011). Cognitive control involves the dlPFC (Koechlin, Ody, & Kouneiher, 2003); the explanation for such a reduction is based on the reduction of cognitive resources in religious rituals in monitoring errors (Schjoedt et al., 2013). This may induce a belief in the mysticalness of specific sensorial experiences on account of a supernatural phenomenon - or partially on account of improper inhibitory control (Hood Jr, Hill, & Spilka, 2018). Lesions of the essential networks in cognitive control (e.g., dlPFC) may affect the human belief system and thus raise paranormal beliefs. 
Research has recently focused on how such psychological properties are neurophysiologically correlated. In the electroencephalogram (EEG) method, alternations in the time of cerebral cortex electrical activity arising from postsynaptic potentials of numerous neurons with the similarity of spatial orientation are recorded (Cassani, Estarellas, San-Martin, Fraga, & Falk, 2018). Such electric potentials are measured by scalp-positioned electrodes. EEG spatial resolution associates with the number of electrodes and position (arrangement) on the scalp. The international 10-20 system is the most used positioning; it involves twenty-one electrode locations. Other variants of this system with larger density are also employed, which involve 64 and 128 electrodes; the former is known as the international 10-10 system, while the latter is referred to as the 10-5 system (Jurcak, Tsuzuki, & Dan, 2007). Other positioning systems include the Maudsley (Seeck et al., 2017) and Geodesics (Hu, Lai, Valdes-Sosa, Bringas-Vega, M. L., & Yao, 2018) layouts. Research has recently demonstrated that quantitative EEG (qEEG) is a valid clinical instrument to diagnose and investigate diseases, mental states, and cortical disorders (Boersma et al., 2011; Jeong, Kim, Song, Chung, & Jeong, 2016; Sponheim, Clementz, Iacono, & Beiser, 2000; Wang et al., 2013). The power/amplitude and network connectivity (including phase delay, asymmetry, and coherence) indices are two categories of EEG variables. They represent the correlation extent of various brain regions (Thatcher, North, & Biver, 2005). 
The EEG measures of coherence can be employed to effectively estimate the connectivity of the cortical brain region (Rippon, Brock, Brown, & Boucher, 2007). The EEG coherence measurement approximates the synchronization of two regions in the brain based on EEG signal records in various scalp sites (Nunez & Srinivasan, 2006). Large EEG coherence stands for the high synchronization of brain regions, while small coherence represents poor synchronization (Murias, Webb, Greenson, & Dawson, 2007). Apart from the synchronicity degree, research has demonstrated that various EEG frequencies are correlated with various cognitive processes. It is possible to use event-associated neural oscillations to analyze neural responses via several frequency bands (such as alpha, theta, gamma, beta, and delta) (Nguyen et al., 2017). Each frequency band transfers distinct information on basic brain processes within paranormal beliefs. Furthermore, several resting-state EEG works demonstrated that the activities of the alpha (Ishii et al., 1999; Nigbur, Ivanova, & Stürmer, 2011; Yamanaka & Yamamoto, 2010), beta (Bresnahan & Barry, 2002; Choi et al., 2013; Kühn et al., 2004; Lee et al., 2017), and gamma (Barry et al., 2010; Benasich, Gou, Choudhury, & Harris, 2008; Han et al., 2020; Harmony, Alba, Marroquín, & González-Frankenberger, 2009; van Wingerden, Vinck, Lankelma, & Pennartz, 2010) bands are related to the frontal region. They suggested that the frontal region could be related to the activities of the gamma, β, and α bands. 
Although paranormal beliefs have not been related to frontal regions in the literature, research has provided electrophysiological findings concerning paranormal beliefs; previous works focused more on the lateralization of the brain in such findings. The source locations of skeptics and paranormal believers were shown to substantially differ in the beta, gamma, and delta bands (Gianotti, Faber, & Lehmann, 2002; Narmashiri, Sohrabi, & Hatami, 2020). Also, right-localizing gamma-band activity was demonstrated to support previous observations of beta-band activity. Several studies argued paranormal believers have greater right-hemisphere activity and lower or irregular asymmetry of the hemispheres (Pizzagalli et al., 2000). In paranormal believers and non-believers, significant beta (excitatory) activity was located to the right and left hemispheres, respectively. Furthermore, compared to those who have low transliminality, high-transliminality people have smaller beta and alpha bands in the right superior temporal cortex and the left occipital/parietal region (Fleck et al., 2008). High transliminality people were reported to have high gamma bands within the media-frontal cortex and low gamma bands within the right temporal lobe and left temporal/occipital region. 
Given that, some band activities, such as the alpha, beta, and gamma in the frontal lobe were associated with cognitive function; therefore, we concentrated on alpha, beta, and gamma bands activity in the present study. Previous studies have shown dysfunction in the frontal lobe in supernatural beliefs and paranormal phenomena (Cristofori et al., 2016; Wain & Spinella, 2007). Therefore, according to the executive inhibition hypothesis, we expected that dysfunction of the frontal lobe would be associated with increased paranormal beliefs due to the frontal cortex (FC) role in regulating inhibition and evidence showing FC regions deactivation in paranormal experiences. Based on this hypothesis, we hypothesized that paranormal beliefs are regulated by key regions in the frontal lobe. We sought to find the characteristics of the resting-state EEG activity to identify frontal region activities in paranormal believers. Due to the lack of resting-state EEG studies, especially in the frontal lobe in paranormal beliefs, we predict that reduction in frontal activity in the alpha, beta, and gamma band (left and right hemispheres) was related to the rise in paranormal beliefs.

2. Methods 
Participants 

A total of 20 (10 girls, Mean±SD age: 22.50±4.07 years, the age range of 19-34 years) healthy right-handed psychology students (Edinburgh handedness inventory) were selected for the study. The participants were recruited from classrooms at the University of Tehran City, Iran. Based on the self-report questionnaire, the participants had no history of mental, neurological, or personality disorders, no acute or chronic diseases, and no history of alcohol or substance abuse, epilepsy, psychosis, head injury, and other mental disorders. 

Behavioral measure 
The revised paranormal belief scale (RPBS) has 26 items. These items are classified into 7 groups, including Psi, traditional religious beliefs, superstition, witchcraft, precognition, extraordinary life forms, and spiritualism. The seven-point Likert scale was used to rate the items, where 1 represented “highly disagree,” while 7 stood for “highly agree” (Tobacyk, 2004). Moreover, to measure reliability, Cronbach’s alpha was employed and calculated to be 0.92. 

Electroencephalogram (EEG) recording
The participants were given seats and rested in a place connected to the recording room. A 5-minute EEG recording was performed with open eyes (Mitsar Medical, Petersburg, Russia). The standard 10-20 layout was applied to seven sites, including Fz, F7, F8, F4, F3, FP1, and FP2, of 32 channels. A single channel consisting of bipolar electrodes was attached to the ears as a reference. The ground electrode was located in the space between Fz and FPz. The signal was sampled at 250 Hz. The impedance of the electrodes was lower than 5 kΩ. Also, the band-pass filtration of the EEG signals was performed using Mitsar 202 at a frequency of 0.1-40 Hz. The recordings of the WinEEG setup were transmitted to the EEGLAB toolbox (NG 2.5.5; Applied Neuroscience, Inc., St. Petersburg, USA) to undergo standard preprocessing. The Artifact rejection toolbox was employed to perform offline artifact removal. Furthermore, the visual inspection of the EEG recordings was carried out to eliminat artifact effects (such as eye muscle movement effects). Then, a rapid Fourier transform was applied in MATLAB to analyze the spectra (MathWorks, Natick, Massachusetts, USA). The cleaned EEG data at 9 frequency bands including, gamma (30-40Hz), beta2 (19-30Hz), beta1 (13-21Hz), beta (13-30Hz), alpha2 (11-13Hz), alpha1 (8-10Hz), alpha (8-13Hz), theta (4-8Hz), and delta (1-4Hz), were utilized to extract the absolute (uV2) power. Based on (Bastos, Adamatti, & Billa, 2016), the activities in seven sites were hypothetically employed as frontal regions (i.e., F8, F4, FP2, Fz, F7, F3, and FP1). This study concentrated on gamma, beta, and alpha frontal region activities to examine the proposed hypotheses. 

Statistical analysis
Before the formal analysis, we conducted an exploratory data analysis to identify and remove outliers to avoid the possibility of spurious results. We used Pearson’s correlations and multiple regression analysis to explore the relationships between the resting-state EEG activities and the paranormal beliefs of the subjects. Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics v. 24 (IBM Inc., New York, USA), and P values less than 0.05 were considered statistically significant. 

3. Result
In the present study, 20 subjects participated and the Mean±SD age was 22.50±4.07; 10 subjects were girls (50%) and 10 subjects were boys (50%); the Mean±SD years of education was 15.50±1.72. The Mean±SD of paranormal beliefs for the participants were 84.80±17.03.
Table 1 presented a negative correlation between the paranormal beliefs and the coherence of alpha in the left hemisphere regions (r=-0.455, P<0.05) (Figure 1).
In the right and left hemispheres, no relationship was observed between paranormal beliefs and EEG coherence in the Δ, ϴ, α1, α2, β, β1, β2, and gamma-band of the regions.
Table 2 presented a negative correlation between paranormal beliefs and the coherence of alpha (r=-0.471, P<0.05), beta1 (r=-0.547, P<0.05), beta2 (r=-0.568, P<0.001), and gamma-band activities (r=-0.453, P<0.05) between the regions in the right hemisphere (Figure 2).


However, in the right hemispheres, no relationship was observed between paranormal beliefs and EEG coherence in Δ, ϴ, α1, α2, and β-band activities of the regions.
Table 3 presented a negative correlation between paranormal beliefs and α (r=-0.503, P<0.05), β1 (r=-0.449, P<0.05), β2 (r=-0.517, P<0.05), and gamma-band activities (r=-0.460, P<0.05) between the regions of the two hemispheres (Figure 3).


In the two hemispheres, no relationship was observed between paranormal beliefs and EEG coherence in delta, theta, alpha1, alpha2, and beta-band activities of the regions.
In the present study, we conducted a regression analysis to identify the effects of volumetric conduction on EEG coherence using average band activities in hemispheres. Table 4 presented that any of the band activities of the frontal lobe were not predictors of paranormal beliefs (R2=0.26, F=1.75, P=0.19). 

Table 5 revealed that the coherence of right hemisphere regions predicted 51% of the total variance of paranormal beliefs (R2=0.51, F=3.19, P<0.04).

In addition, α (b=-1.19, P<0.03), α1 (b=-1.59, P<0.01), β (b=-0.73, P<0.02), and β2 (b=-1.85, P<0.04) coherence at the left frontal regions negatively predicted paranormal beliefs. 
The results of Table 6 revealed that the coherence of two hemisphere regions (interhemispheric) predicted 58% of the total variance of paranormal beliefs (adjusted R2=0.58, F=4.02, P<0.02). In addition, delta (b=-0.455, P<0.02), α1 (b=-0.74, P<0.01), and β (b=-0.72, P<0.01), coherence at the interhemispheric (two hemispheres) negatively predicted paranormal beliefs. 

4. Discussion 
The present study aimed to examine the correlation between the EEG coherence over frontal regions and paranormal beliefs. In the left hemisphere, the results of this study showed that paranormal beliefs were negatively related to the EEG coherence of alpha band activity. In addition, paranormal beliefs were associated with EEG coherence of α and β1, β2, and gamma-band activities in the right frontal lobe. We found a negative correlation between paranormal beliefs and the EEG coherence of α and β1, β2, and gamma band activities between the frontal lobe of the two hemispheres. Additionally, EEG coherence of α, α1, β, and β2 band activities between the right frontal lobe and EEG coherence of Δ, α1, and band activities in the frontal lobe of the two hemispheres predicted paranormal beliefs.
The findings expectedly revealed that low frontal region activity is negatively related to paranormal believers. Previous works observed paranormal beliefs and phenomena to be associated with functionality reduction in frontal regions, particularly the prefrontal cortex (Cristofori et al., 2016; Wain & Spinella, 2007). These findings are consistent with the executive inhibition hypothesis. Previous works demonstrated a relationship between the dysfunction of the frontal region and increased paranormal experience. These findings can support this hypothesis via the mechanism proposed for the association of frontal lobe activity dysfunction with supernatural beliefs, resulting in increasing supernatural beliefs. 
Decreased gamma, beta, and alpha-band activity in the frontal regions and the paranormal belief enhancement can be related to executive frontal dysfunction in paranormal beliefs. According to the findings, alpha power implies a neural inhibitory mechanism in external sensory information influx in resting-state EEG (Mathewson et al., 2011). Based on alpha power, such a neural mechanism can be associated with the gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA)ergic inhibitory activity via a brain inter-neuronal network (Jensen & Mazaheri, 2010) as well as unbalanced glutamatergic-GABAergic neurotransmission. Furthermore, when no external stimuli exist, dysfunction of resting-state beta-band activity could imply dopamine depletion in the dlPFC region (George et al., 2013), which associates with the executive inhibitory function. Also, reduced gamma-band activity can be associated with front lobe neural dysfunction (Rosanova et al., 2009) “mendeley”:{“formattedCitation”; (Rosanova et al., 2009), and impairment reflection in pyramidal neuron synchronous inhibition (Gonzalez-Burgos & Lewis, 2008), and GABAergic neurotransmission (Lewis, Hashimoto, & Volk, 2005). 
Nonetheless, the aforementioned executive function theories support previous works identifying inhibitory deregulation as a driver of paranormal experience. Future studies can investigate the possibility of reporting paranormal experiences by individuals. The findings of the present study are not only consistent with mysticism executive neuropsychological theories but also consistent with a wider range of executive brain function studies. dlPFC is essential for the control of emotional and attentional cognitive mechanisms (Davidson, Putnam, & Larson, 2000). In this respect, a large number of works demonstrated that the dlPFC contributes to the down-regulation of emotional processing. Neuroimaging research has shown that dlPFC activity rises under neural stimulus awareness and suppression of fearful stimulus (Amting, Greening, & Mitchell, 2010). In addition, previous studies employed functional and or electrophysiological methods and showed that the dlPFC activity consistently enhanced in re-evaluation (Ochsner et al., 2004). Considering the dlPFC contribution to rationality and emotional control, it is argued that dlPFC plays a key role in regulating mystical experience via the binding of rational experience, deliberation, and context to posterior perceptual phenomenon-influenced cortices. Consequently, mystical experiences can enjoy more contextual search and thus weaken the prefrontal cortical representational process. A damaged superior PFC leads subjects to imagine that their perceptual experience is not in the real world and to adopt imaginary supernatural/mystical explanations (Bulbulia, 2009). 
We had several limitations in this study. Firstly, convenient sampling and selection of the sample from university students constituted one of the limitations of the present study. Secondly, the sample size in the present study was small. Further investigations with a larger population focusing on other variables associated with frontal activity in paranormal beliefs such as executive function are necessary. Thirdly, paranormal beliefs were assessed with a self-report scale. Future studies using objective methods to assess paranormal beliefs level, including cognitive and behavioral function tasks, are needed. In addition, concerning the participation of women in this study, some studies have reported that the function of the prefrontal cortex may be affected by the levels of estrogen and progesterone hormones (Amin, Epperson, Constable, & Canli, 2006; Solis-Ortiz, Guevara, & Corsi-Cabrera, 2004). Therefore, future studies are recommended to control the menstrual period of female participants.

5. Conclusion
Overall, the findings suggest that high paranormal beliefs are associated with reduced EEG alpha-band activity coherence and reduced EEG gamma, beta, and alpha-band activity coherence in the right and left hemispheres, respectively. Such findings imply that paranormal beliefs are related to the activity of the lower frontal region, particularly in the right hemisphere and two hemispheres. The present work supports the association of paranormal beliefs with executive brain functions and claims that frontal brain functions are likely to influence paranormal beliefs.

Ethical Considerations
Compliance with ethical guidelines

There were no ethical considerations to be considered in this research.

Funding
This study was supported by the Cognitive Sciences and Technologies Council (Grant No.: 8349). This study was extracted from the PhD dissertation. 

Authors' contributions
Conceptualization, methodology, software, writing-original draft, writing-review, and editing: Abdolvahed Narmashiri; Conceptualization, methodology, writing-review and editing, supervision: Javad Hatami; Methodology, software, writing-review and editing, supervision: Reza Khosrowabadi; Resources, supervision: Ahmad Sohrabi.

Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments
The funder had no role in the research. The funders had no role in study design, data collection, analysis, decision to publish, or manuscript preparation. We thank the participants for their cooperation in this research.

References 
Amin, Z., Epperson, C. N., Constable, R. T., & Canli, T. (2006). Effects of estrogen variation on neural correlates of emotional response inhibition. NeuroImage, 32(1), 457-464. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.013] [PMID]
Amting, J. M., Greening, S. G., & Mitchell, D. G. V. (2010). Multiple mechanisms of consciousness: the neural correlates of emotional awareness. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(30), 10039-10047. [DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6434-09.2010] [PMID] [PMCID]
Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 170-177. [DOI:10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010] [PMID]
Barry, R. J., Clarke, A. R., Hajos, M., McCarthy, R., Selikowitz, M., & Dupuy, F. E. (2010). Resting-state EEG gamma activity in children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 121(11), 1871-1877. [DOI:10.1016/j.clinph.2010.04.022] [PMID]
Bastos, N. S., Adamatti, D. F., & Billa, C. Z. (2016). Discovering patterns in brain signals using decision trees. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, 2016, 1-10. [DOI:10.1155/2016/6391807] [PMID] [PMCID]
Benasich, A. A., Gou, Z., Choudhury, N., & Harris, K. D. (2008). Early cognitive and language skills are linked to resting frontal gamma power across the first 3 years. Behavioural Brain Research, 195(2), 215-222. [DOI:10.1016/j.bbr.2008.08.049] [PMID] [PMCID]
Betsch, T., Aßmann, L., & Glöckner, A. (2020). Paranormal beliefs and individual differences: story seeking without reasoned review. Heliyon, 6(6), e04259. [DOI:10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e04259] [PMID] [PMCID]
Betsch, T., Jäckel, P., Hammes, M., & Brinkmann, B. J. (2021). On the Adaptive Value of Paranormal Beliefs-a Qualitative Study. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 1-11. [DOI:10.1007/s12124-020-09594-5] [PMID] [PMCID]
Boersma, M., Smit, D. J. A., de Bie, H. M. A., Van Baal, G. C. M., Boomsma, D. I., de Geus, E. J. C., et al. (2011). Network analysis of resting state EEG in the developing young brain: Structure comes with maturation. Human Brain Mapping, 32(3), 413-425. [DOI:10.1002/hbm.21030] [PMID] [PMCID]
Bresnahan, S. M., & Barry, R. J. (2002). Specificity of quantitative EEG analysis in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Research, 112(2), 133-144. [DOI:10.1016/S0165-1781(02)00190-7]
Bulbulia, J. (2009). Religiosity as mental time-travel. The Believing Primate, 44-75. [DOI:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199557028.003.0003]
Cassani, R., Estarellas, M., San-Martin, R., Fraga, F. J., & Falk, T. H. (2018). Systematic review on resting-state EEG for Alzheimer’s disease diagnosis and progression assessment. Disease Markers, 2018. [DOI:10.1155/2018/5174815] [PMID] [PMCID]
Choi, J.-S., Park, S. M., Lee, J., Hwang, J. Y., Jung, H. Y., Choi, S.-W., et al. (2013). Resting-state beta and gamma activity in Internet addiction. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 89(3), 328-333. [DOI:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.007] [PMID]
Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201-215. [DOI:10.1038/nrn755] [PMID]
Cristofori, I., Bulbulia, J., Shaver, J. H., Wilson, M., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2016). Neural correlates of mystical experience. Neuropsychologia, 80, 212-220. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.11.021] [PMID]
Davidson, R. J., Putnam, K. M., & Larson, C. L. (2000). Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation: A possible prelude to violence. Science, 289(5479), 591-594. [DOI:10.1126/science.289.5479.591] [PMID]
Deeley, P. Q. (2003). Social, cognitive, and neural constraints on subjectivity and agency: Implications for dissociative identity disorder. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 10(2), 161-167. [DOI:10.1353/ppp.2003.0095]
Drinkwater, K., Denovan, A., Dagnall, N., & Parker, A. (2020). Predictors of hearing electronic voice phenomena in random noise: Schizotypy, fantasy proneness, and paranormal beliefs. Journal of Parapsychology, 84(1), 96-113. [DOI:10.30891/jopar.2020.01.09]
FioRito, T. A., Abeyta, A. A., & Routledge, C. (2020). Religion, paranormal beliefs, and meaning in life. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 1-8. [DOI:10.1080/2153599X.2020.1824938]
Fleck, J., Green, D., Stevenson, J., Payne, L., Bowden, E., Jungbeeman, M., et al. (2008). The transliminal brain at rest: Baseline EEG, unusual experiences, and access to unconscious mental activity. Cortex, 44(10), 1353-1363. [DOI:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.024] [PMID] [PMCID]
George, J. S., Strunk, J., Mak-McCully, R., Houser, M., Poizner, H., & Aron, A. R. (2013). Dopaminergic therapy in Parkinson’s disease decreases cortical beta band coherence in the resting state and increases cortical beta band power during executive control. NeuroImage: Clinical, 3, 261-270. [DOI:10.1016/j.nicl.2013.07.013] [PMID] [PMCID]
Gianotti, L. R. R., Faber, P. L., & Lehmann, D. (2002). EEG source locations after guessed random events in believers and skeptics of paranormal phenomena. International Congress Series, 1232, 439-441. [DOI:10.1016/S0531-5131(01)00681-1]
Gonzalez-Burgos, G., & Lewis, D. A. (2008). GABA neurons and the mechanisms of network oscillations: Implications for understanding cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(5), 944-961. [DOI:10.1093/schbul/sbn070] [PMID] [PMCID]
Han, Y., Dai, Z., Ridwan, M. C., Lin, P., Zhou, H., Wang, H., et al. (2020). Connectivity of the frontal cortical oscillatory dynamics underlying inhibitory control during a go/no-go task as a predictive biomarker in major depression. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, 707. [DOI:10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00707] [PMID] [PMCID]
Harmony, T., Alba, A., Marroquín, J. L., & González-Frankenberger, B. (2009). Time-frequency-topographic analysis of induced power and synchrony of EEG signals during a go/no-go task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 71(1), 9-16. [DOI:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.020] [PMID]
Hood Jr, R. W., Hill, P. C., & Spilka, B. (2018). The psychology of religion: An empirical approach. Guilford Publications. [Link]
Hu, S., Lai, Y., Valdes-Sosa, P. A., Bringas-Vega, M. L., & Yao, D. (2018). How do reference montage and electrodes setup affect the measured scalp EEG potentials? Journal of Neural Engineering, 15(2), 26013. [DOI:10.1088/1741-2552/aaa13f]
Ishii, R., Shinosaki, K., Ukai, S., Inouye, T., Ishihara, T., Yoshimine, T., et al. (1999). Medial prefrontal cortex generates frontal midline theta rhythm. NeuroReport, 10(4), 675-679. [DOI:10.1097/00001756-199903170-00003] [PMID]
Jensen, O., & Mazaheri, A. (2010). Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: Gating by inhibition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 186. [DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186]
Jeong, D.-H., Kim, Y.-D., Song, I.-U., Chung, Y.-A., & Jeong, J. (2016). Wavelet energy and wavelet coherence as EEG biomarkers for the diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease-related dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Entropy, 18(1), 8. [DOI:10.3390/e18010008]
Jurcak, V., Tsuzuki, D., & Dan, I. (2007). 10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 systems revisited: Their validity as relative head-surface-based positioning systems. Neuroimage, 34(4), 1600-1611. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.024] [PMID]
Koechlin, E., Ody, C., & Kouneiher, F. (2003). The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science, 302(5648), 1181-1185. [DOI:10.1126/science.1088545] [PMID]
Kühn, A. A., Williams, D., Kupsch, A., Limousin, P., Hariz, M., Schneider, G., et al. (2004). Event‐related beta desynchronization in human subthalamic nucleus correlates with motor performance. Brain, 127(4), 735-746. [DOI:10.1093/brain/awh106] [PMID]
Lee, J. Y., Park, S. M., Kim, Y. J., Kim, D. J., Choi, S.-W., Kwon, J. S., et al. (2017). Resting-state EEG activity related to impulsivity in gambling disorder. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(3), 387-395. [DOI:10.1556/2006.6.2017.055] [PMID] [PMCID]
Lewis, D. A., Hashimoto, T., & Volk, D. W. (2005). Cortical inhibitory neurons and schizophrenia. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(4), 312-324. [DOI:10.1038/nrn1648] [PMID]
Lindeman, M., & Aarnio, K. (2007). Superstitious, magical, and paranormal beliefs: An integrative model. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(4), 731-744. [DOI:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.06.009]
Lindeman, M., & Saher, M. (2007). Vitalism, purpose and superstition. British Journal of Psychology, 98(1), 33-44. [DOI:10.1348/000712606X101808] [PMID]
Lindeman, M., Svedholm, A. M., Riekki, T., Raij, T., & Hari, R. (2013). Is it just a brick wall or a sign from the universe? An fMRI study of supernatural believers and skeptics. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(8), 943-949. [DOI:10.1093/scan/nss096] [PMID] [PMCID]
Mathewson, K. E., Lleras, A., Beck, D. M., Fabiani, M., Ro, T., & Gratton, G. (2011). Pulsed out of awareness: EEG alpha oscillations represent a pulsed-inhibition of ongoing cortical processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 99. [DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00099] [PMID] [PMCID]
Murias, M., Webb, S. J., Greenson, J., & Dawson, G. (2007). Resting state cortical connectivity reflected in EEG coherence in individuals with autism. Biological Psychiatry, 62(3), 270-273. [DOI:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.012] [PMID] [PMCID]
Narmashiri, A., Hatami, J., Khosrowabadi, R., & Sohrabi, A. (2021). The role of cognitive control in paranormal beliefs: a study based on performance in go/no-go task. BCN. accepted. [DOI:10.32598/bcn.2021.923.3]
Narmashiri, A., Sohrabi, A., & Hatami, J. (2017). Perceptual-cognitive biases in relation to paranormal beliefs: A comparative study in Brain lateralization groups. Journal of Neuropsychology, 2(7), 77-90. https://www.sid.ir/en/journal/ViewPaper.aspx?id=603830
Narmashiri, A., Sohrabi, A., & Hatami, J. (2020). Brainwave pattern in paranormal beliefs: An EEG-based study in Severe and Mild groups. Journal of Neuropsychology, 5(19), 89-98. [DOI:10.30473/CLPSY.2020.48062.1455]
Narmashiri, A., Sohrabi, A., Hatami, J., Amirfakhraei, A., & Haghighat, S. (2019). Investigating the role of brain lateralization and gender in paranormal beliefs. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience Journal, 10(6), 589-596. [DOI:10.32598/bcn.9.10.923.1] [PMID] [PMCID]
Newberg, A. B., Wintering, N. A., Morgan, D., & Waldman, M. R. (2006). The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during glossolalia: A preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 148(1), 67-71. [DOI:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.07.001] [PMID]
Nguyen, L. T., Mudar, R. A., Chiang, H.-S., Schneider, J. M., Maguire, M. J., Kraut, M. A., , et al. (2017). Theta and alpha alterations in amnestic mild cognitive impairment in semantic go/nogo tasks. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9, 160. [DOI:10.3389/fnagi.2017.00160] [PMID] [PMCID]
Nigbur, R., Ivanova, G., & Stürmer, B. (2011). Theta power as a marker for cognitive interference. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122(11), 2185-2194. [DOI:10.1016/j.clinph.2011.03.030] [PMID]
Nunez, P. L., & Srinivasan, R. (2006). Electric fields of the brain: The neurophysics of EEG. Oxford University Press, USA. [DOI:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195050387.001.0001]
Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., , et al. (2004). For better or for worse: Neural systems supporting the cognitive down-and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage, 23(2), 483-499. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.030] [PMID]
Pizzagalli, D., Lehmann, D., Gianotti, L., Koenig, T., Tanaka, H., Wackermann, J., et al. (2000). Brain electric correlates of strong belief in paranormal phenomena: Intracerebral EEG source and regional Omega complexity analyses. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 100(3), 139-154. [DOI:10.1016/S0925-4927(00)00070-6]
Rippon, G., Brock, J., Brown, C., & Boucher, J. (2007). Disordered connectivity in the autistic brain: challenges for the ‘new psychophysiology.’ International Journal of Psychophysiology, 63(2), 164-172. [DOI:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.03.012] [PMID]
Rosanova, M., Casali, A., Bellina, V., Resta, F., Mariotti, M., & Massimini, M. (2009). Natural frequencies of human corticothalamic circuits. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(24), 7679-7685. [DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0445-09.2009] [PMID] [PMCID]
Schjoedt, U., Sørensen, J., Nielbo, K. L., Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). Cognitive resource depletion in religious interactions. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 3(1), 39-55. [DOI:10.1080/2153599X.2012.736714]
Schjoedt, U., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Geertz, A. W., Lund, T. E., & Roepstorff, A. (2011). The power of charisma-perceived charisma inhibits the frontal executive network of believers in intercessory prayer. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(1), 119-127. [DOI:10.1093/scan/nsq023] [PMID] [PMCID]
Seeck, M., Koessler, L., Bast, T., Leijten, F., Michel, C., Baumgartner, et al. (2017). The standardized EEG electrode array of the IFCN. Clinical Neurophysiology, 128(10), 2070-2077. [DOI:10.1016/j.clinph.2017.06.254] [PMID]
Solis-Ortiz, S., Guevara, M., & Corsi-Cabrera, M. (2004). Performance in a test demanding prefrontal functions is favored by early luteal phase progesterone: an electroencephalographic study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29(8), 1047-1057. [DOI:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2003.10.007] [PMID]
Sponheim, S. R., Clementz, B. A., Iacono, W. G., & Beiser, M. (2000). Clinical and biological concomitants of resting state EEG power abnormalities in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 48(11), 1088-1097. [DOI:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00907-0]
Thatcher, R. W., North, D., & Biver, C. (2005). EEG and intelligence: Relations between EEG coherence, EEG phase delay and power. Clinical Neurophysiology, 116(9), 2129-2141. [DOI:10.1016/j.clinph.2005.04.026] [PMID]
Tobacyk, J. J. (2004). A revised paranormal belief scale. International Journal of Transpersonal Studies, 23(1), 94-98. [DOI:10.24972/ijts.2004.23.1.94]
van Wingerden, M., Vinck, M., Lankelma, J. V, & Pennartz, C. M. A. (2010). Learning-associated gamma-band phase-locking of action-outcome selective neurons in orbitofrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(30), 10025-10038. [DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0222-10.2010] [PMID] [PMCID]
Wain, O., & Spinella, M. (2007). Executive functions in morality, religion, and paranormal beliefs. International Journal of Neuroscience, 117(1), 135-146. [DOI:10.1080/00207450500534068] [PMID]
Wang, J., Barstein, J., Ethridge, L. E., Mosconi, M. W., Takarae, Y., & Sweeney, J. A. (2013). Resting state EEG abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 5(1), 24. [DOI:10.1186/1866-1955-5-24] [PMID] [PMCID]
Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1998). Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains.
Wilson, J. A. (2018). Reducing pseudoscientific and paranormal beliefs in university students through a course in science and critical thinking. Science & Education, 27(1-2), 183-210. [DOI:10.1007/s11191-018-9956-0]
Yamanaka, K., & Yamamoto, Y. (2010). Single-trial EEG power and phase dynamics associated with voluntary response inhibition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(4), 714-727. [DOI:10.1162/jocn.2009.21258] [PMID]
Type of Study: Original | Subject: Cognitive Neuroscience
Received: 2021/01/3 | Accepted: 2021/01/30 | Published: 2022/07/24

Add your comments about this article : Your username or Email:
CAPTCHA

Send email to the article author


Rights and permissions
Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

© 2024 CC BY-NC 4.0 | Basic and Clinical Neuroscience

Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb