Volume 9, Issue 6 (November & December 2018)                   BCN 2018, 9(6): 448-457 | Back to browse issues page


XML Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Yazdi-Ravandii S, Shamsaei F, Matinnia N, Shams J, Moghimbeigi A, Ghaleiha A et al . Cognitive Process in Patients With Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: A Cross-Sectional Analytic Study. BCN 2018; 9 (6) :448-457
URL: http://bcn.iums.ac.ir/article-1-1043-en.html
1- Behavioral Disorders and Substance Abuse Research Center, Hamadan University of Medical Sciences, Hamadan, Iran.
2- Department of Nursing, Faculty of Basic Sciences, Hamadan Branch, Islamic Azad University, Hamadan, Iran.
3- Behavioral Sciences Research Center, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran.
4- Modeling of Noncommunicable Disease Research Center, Department of Biostatistics, School of Health, Hamadan University of Medical Sciences, Hamadan, Iran.
Full-Text [PDF 744 kb]       |   Abstract (HTML) 
Full-Text:  
1. Introduction
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD) is currently recognized as one of the disabling mental illnesses. “It is characterized by recurrent, intrusive, and unwanted thoughts (obsession), impulses, and images, often associated with compulsive behaviors that are repetitive, ritualized and often time consuming” (American Psychiatric Publication, 2013). The usual obsessions include contamination, doubting, ordering/symmetry, aggressive thoughts, religion, and sexual imagery (Benzina, Mallet,  Burguière, N’diaye, & Pelissolo, 2016).
Several research studies have reported that patients with OCD have many deficits in cognitive flexibility, set shifting, attention and memory (Tükel et al., 2012; Aydın, Koybasi, Sert, Mete, & Oyekcin, 2014). Although dysfunctions in cognitive abilities are obvious in the daily behavior of OCD patients, fundamental research on neurocognitive dysfunction is still ongoing, and the dimensions of this type of dysfunction are unknown (Chamberlain, Blackwell, Fineberg, Robbins, & Sahakian, 2005). Among all cognitive impairments, deficiencies in information processing and executive functions greatly restrain patient’s abilities to preserve, acquire and relearn the required skills for suitable performances (Keefe, 1995). 
Cognitive process dysfunction hypotheses have been used to explain many psychological disorders such as OCD (Penadés, Catalán, Andrés, Salamero, & Gastó, 2005), schizophrenia (Moritz et al., 2002), autism (Tanguay, 2000), and bipolar disorders (Balanzá-Martínez et al., 2010). 
Based on different studies, patients with OCD demonstrated specific cognitive deficits on attention, executive and memory functions tasks (Rotge et al., 2008; Benzina et al., 2016). Patients with OCD have an impaired performance pattern, which is qualitatively like the performance of patients with frontal lobe excisions and subcortical pathology. In addition, several research studies reported abnormalities in the cortico-basal ganglion circuits in OCD (Rotge et al., 2008Benzina et al., 2016).
Converging findings from imaging research implicate abnormalities in frontal-subcortical circuitry in the OCD pathogenesis (Saxena & Rauch, 2000; Lewin et al., 2014). Functional and structural imaging in OCD showed hyperactivity in the limbic, associative cortical area, and related basal ganglia (Menzies et al., 2008). Recent studies recommend that neurobiological abnormalities play a main role in the cognitive dysfunction of these patients (Abramovitch, Abramowitz, & Mittelman, 2013; Benzina et al., 2016).
Many neurocognitive studies suggest abnormalities in the cognitive process of individuals with OCD (Chamberlain et al., 2007; Remijnse et al., 2006; Viswanath, Janardhan Reddy, Kumar, Kandavel, & Chandrashekar, 2009). However, not all studies have found abnormalities and when abnormalities are detected, the results are not always consistent (Moritz, Kloss, & Jelinek, 2010).
Although cognitive processes have been investigated in OCD and promising results have been obtained, sometimes these findings were controversial. Considering the mentioned issues, the identification of various aspects of the cognitive performances of these patients can contribute in better understanding of the existing viewpoints on this disorder. The evaluation of cognitive processes (executive functions and information processing) can be applied in the treatment of these patients. This study aimed to compare the cognitive process in OCD patients and healthy control individuals. The authors hypothesized that OCD patients would perform weaker than healthy control individuals in the executive function and information processing areas.

2. Methods
This cross-sectional descriptive-analytical research was conducted in Farshchian-Sina Hospital of Hamadan City, Iran in 2017. This study included 43 patients who met the DSM-IV-TR criteria for OCD (50 patients were interviewed, seven of whom did not meet the study inclusion criteria). The control group consisted of 43 healthy participants who were selected from Farshchian Hospital and Hamadan University of Medical Sciences personnel and students, who matched the patients in terms of gender, age and education. 
The participants of the two groups were selected by convenience sampling method. Patients in the current study were included as per the following criteria: 1. Having diagnosed with OCD by a psychiatrist according to the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Clinical Version (Persian edition); 2. Being 18 to 65 years old; 3. Having at least elementary level of education; 4. Taking a score of at least 16 on the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS); and 5. Lacking any current psychiatric disorder except diagnosis of OCD. 
Patients were not included in the study if they had 1. A history of or current drug and/or alcohol abuse or dependency (self-report); 2. Any neurologic disease or concomitant general medical condition; 3. Serious head injury history; 4. Intellectual disability or any clinical condition that could affect cognitive performance; 5. Significant medical illness; 6. Electroconvulsive therapy in the last year; 7. Physical disability (e.g. blindness, deafness, speech problems, paralysis, amputation); and 8. Chosen to withdraw from the study. 

2.1. Clinical measures 
The sociodemographic profile included name, age, gender, address, marital status, and education.

2.1.1. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Disorders 
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Disorders (SCID-I) is a structured interview method that provides information to diagnose Axis-I psychiatric disorders included in DSM (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1997). In the current study, the Persian version was used. Administration of this test usually takes about 45 to 90 minutes in a single session (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1996). Reliability and validity of SCID have been reported acceptable in the Iranian clinical population (Sharifi et al., 2009).

2.1.2. Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale 
Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS) was developed by Goodman and some other researchers to assess the severity of OCD symptoms (Goodman et al., 1989). Y-BOCS includes 10 self-report items. The first 5 questions relate to obsession, and the other 5 questions relate to compulsion. All items have a score ranging from 0 (symptoms free) to 4 (extreme symptoms). The scores were classified as subclinical (0 to 7), mild (8 to 15), moderate (16 to 23), severe (24 to 33), and extreme (34 to 40). Moreover, Rajezi Esfahani, Motaghipour, Kamkari, Zahiredin and Janbozorgi (2012) studied the validity and reliability of the Persian version of Y-BOCS. They claimed that its test-retest reliability coefficients, split-half reliability, and internal consistency ranged between 0.89 and 0.99.

2.2. Cognitive process assessment
2.2.1. Wisconsin Cart Sorting Test 

Wisconsin Cart Sorting Test (WCST) measures the executive functions such as flexibility, strategic planning, goal-oriented behavior, and organized searching. Assessing the shift depends on the concepts, and forming abstractions are the main purposes of this test (Spreen & Strauss, 1998). Moreover, a computerized version of a 64-card WCST was used. WCST has 64 cards, containing figures in different shapes: Triangle, star, cross and circle (numbered from 1 to 4). The color of these shapes are red, green, yellow and blue. Therefore, each card has a figure (one of the four shapes), a number (1 to 4), and a color (red, green, yellow or blue). 
The combination of these choices provides 64 different variations. For scoring, we considered the following: score of perseveration errors (this error arises when the subject, despite receiving feedback from the assessor, is trying to avert the incorrect response or continues sorting based on the former principle or incorrect guess), number of correct and incorrect responses, trials to first complete category, time of completion (second), and the number of completed categories (which identifies correct sorting according to figures, colors, and numbers). 
In this test, higher number of completed categories were considered as a better abstractive capability and lower perseverative error, and higher conceptual level response scores indicated less mental rigidity and better cognitive flexibility (Tükel et al., 2012). Naderi (1994) by doing test-retest reported the reliability of the test as 0.85 between Iranian populations.

2.3. Information processing assessment
2.3.1. Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test

To assess the rate of attention and information processing, Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) is used as a serial addition task. In this test, 61 single-digit numbers between 1 and 9 are read randomly to the patients in fixed-time intervals in two versions, i.e. in 3 and 2 seconds. In the current study, both versions were administered. 
The patient has to sum each number to the one immediately prior to it. For example, if numbers 4 and 7 are presented, the correct answer would be 11. Higher number of correct answers indicate better performance. A maximum of 60 correct responses can be achieved (Sherman, Strauss, & Spellacy, 1997). Some researchers have indicated that PASAT has a high internal consistency, typically ranged between 0.76 and 0.95 (Macleod & Prior, 1996; Sherman et al., 1997). 

2.4. Statistical analysis
SPSS Version 22.00 was used for all statistical analyses. Kolmogorov-Smirnov was used to test data normality. The difference between the two groups in numerical variables was evaluated by using the Independent samples t test. If the mentioned assumptions were not met, Mann–Whitney U test was used. For categorical variables, χ2 test or Fisher exact test was utilized (Cohen, 1988). Statistics for effect size was calculated for each cognitive measure. Furthermore, to investigate the relationship between the cognitive test and Y-BOCS scores in the OCD group, the Pearson correlation was used.

3. Results
The participants’ ages ranged between 19 and 62 years with the Mean±SD age of 35.12±11.00 years in the OCD group, and 32.35±8.81 years in the healthy group. Table 1 presents demographic variables of the two groups and the scores of obsession, compulsion, and total score of Y-BOCS in the OCD group. There was no significant difference in gender, age, and educational and marital status between the two groups based on Chi-square and Fisher exact tests.
Table 2 presents all the descriptive indices of WSCT in two groups. As it can be seen, compared to healthy control individuals, patients with OCD performed worse in all WCST subtest scores. For example, the Mean±SD of the perseverative error and category were respectively 12.93±7.18 and 2.05±1.44 in the OCD group  and 4.95±4.07 and 4.07±1.56 in the control group. In addition to these indicators, in this study, the time spent on the test was also studied as an indicator. As shown in Table 2, OCD patients spent more time to do the test tasks.
Independent t test results indicate that the performance of patients with OCD was significantly worse than the healthy control individuals with regard to the perseverative error (P<0.001, Cohen’s d=1.367), category (P<0.001, Cohen’s d=1.365), correct response (P<0.001, Cohen’s d=1.306), error response (P<0.001, Cohen’s d=1.738), trials to first complete category (P=0.003, Cohen’s d=0.654), and time of completion (P<0.001, Cohen’s d=0.115). In addition, in the subscale of the conceptual level responses, the Mann-Whitney U test results indicate a lower performance by OCD individuals (P<0.001, Cohen’s d=1.374). According to Table 3, large effect sizes (1.115 to 1.738) were responsible for the differences between the OCD and the healthy groups, expect trials to first complete category that had medium effect sizes. 
In order to investigate information processing, PASAT3’’ and PASAT2’’ were conducted. According to Table 4, the Mean±SD of incorrect responses of the OCD group in the PASAT3’’ and PASAT2’’ test were respectively 16.88±11.14 and 19.26±12.66, indicating that participants of the healthy control group had a better performance in this test. Table 4 presents a significant difference in information processing between the two groups for PASAT3’’ (P<0.001, Cohen’s d=0.784) and PASAT2’’ (P<0.003, Cohen’s d=0.662). Cohen’s d effect sizes between the two groups were medium.


 


 

As observed in Table 5, there was a considerable correlation between obsession, compulsion, and total score of Y-BOCS with the number of completed category (P<0.05), correct responses (P<0.05), error responses (P<0.05) and conceptual level responses (P<0.01) of WCST. Our findings suggest that the correlation between time of completion with obsession and total score was significant (P<0.05). Moreover, correlation between PASAT3’’ and PASAT2’’ with obsession subscale of Y-BOCS was significant.


 


 


 


4. Discussion
The main findings in our investigation revealed a significant difference in information processing and executive function in OCD individuals compared to the healthy control individuals. In the present study, an impairment in cognitive processing was found with the WCST test in patients with OCD. Performance of OCD patients in the present study was weaker than healthy control individuals. They showed more preservative error, error responses, trial to first complete category, time of completion; and fewer categories, correct response, conceptual level response. 
This observation agrees with many studies that have reported deficits in the performances of OCD patients in WCST (Penadés et al., 2005; De Geus, Denys, Sitskoorn,  & Westenberg, 2007; Ghassemzadeh et al., 2012; Aydın et al., 2014). It suggests that OCD patients have a lower cognitive flexibility/set shifting, are more prone to distraction, and inadequate in abstraction ability (Tükel et al., 2012). Since the cognitive flexibility/set shifting is required to understand abstractive principles, it is difficult for OCD patients to hypothesize and assess it, so it seems that they usually have doubt about everything (Saremi, Shariat, Nazari, & Dolatshahi, 2017). 
It is rational to clarify inflexible and persistent thoughts and behaviors of the executive function concept in OCD patients (Saremi et al., 2017). Cognitive deficits were found in patients with OCD, are associated with prefrontal-striatal dysfunction (Greisberg & McKay, 2003; Lacerda et al., 2003). To be more specific, the brain structures that are mostly related to OCD disorders are the basal ganglia structures and orbitofrontal cortex (De Geus et al., 2007). Perseverative behavior is the most prominent feature of the executive/frontal dysfunction (De Geus et al., 2007). The deficits detected in the present patients fit this profile. 

 


 


 

The patients with OCD showed a lower number of completed category and conceptual level responses compared with healthy control individuals, and this illustrates a weaker concept formation. Previous studies have also concluded the same results (Rao, Reddy, Kumar, Kandavel, & Chandrashekar, 2008). However, some research studies report that the WCST score of these patients and the healthy individuals do not differ significantly (Deckersbach et al., 2000; Moritz et al., 2002). This difference in previous research can be due to factors such as patient’s clinical situation, small sample size, intelligence quotient, and medication effects. Another explanation for the present findings on the WCST can be attributed to the attention system dysfunction (Greve, Williams, Haas, Littell, & Reinoso, 1996; Greve, Ingram, & Bianchini, 1998).
Regarding the PASAT, the authors found information processing deficits in patients with OCD in comparison with the healthy control individuals. Patients with OCD often report sensory intolerances, which may lead to important functional impairments. There have been only a few OCD investigations on the stimulus elements’ encoding process, such as attention (Foa & McNally, 1986; Lavy, Van Oppen, & Van Den Hout, 1994). Cognitive theories suggest that OCD should similarly feature abnormal attentional processing toward concern-related materials (Rachman, 1997; Tata, Leibowitz, Prunty, Cameron, & Pickering, 1996; Morein-Zamir et al., 2013). 
This deficiency could be related to anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction, a region involved in conflict monitoring in attention functions and information processing (Devinsky, Morrell, & Vogt, 1995; Van Veen & Carter, 2002). Furthermore, obtaining the effect size of Cohen’s d could lead to more reliable and deeper conception about the cases. Based on Cohen’s (1988) definition of effect size, the results of the current study demonstrated a large effect size in all subscales of WCST and medium effect in PASAT3′′ and PASAT2′′. Effect size values, in accordance with the significance level (P), were evaluated for all cognitive variables, which indicate significant differences between OCD patients and healthy groups. These results showed that healthy control individuals performed better than patients with OCD. Prior meta-analytical research studies presented different effect sizes of various cognitive process subdomains (Abramovitch et al., 2013; Shin, Lee, Kim, & Kwon, 2014; Snyder, Kaiser, Warren, & Heller, 2015).
Our findings showed a considerable negative relation between obsession, compulsion, and total Y-BOCS score with most subscales of the WCST test. PASAT3′′ and PASAT2′′ scores correlated significantly with obsession. Inconsistent with our result, several research findings have reported no significant correlation between the severity of OCD symptoms and performance in cognitive tests (Saremi et al., 2017; Bédard, Joyal, Godbout, & Chantal, 2009). Also, other studies reported a significant relation between the cognitive process and severity of OCD symptoms (Nedeljkovic et al., 2009; Abramovitch, Dar, Schweiger, & Hermesh, 2011). In fact, the severity of OCD symptoms could probably reduce the ability of these patients in their cognitive processes. 
In conclusion, this findings indicate that individuals with OCD suffer from a deficiency in various aspects of the cognitive process such as memory, shifting attention, flexibility, perseveration, and information processing, and they perform more poorly compared to normal individuals. In the current study, some aspects of the cognitive process were reviewed, too. These findings have significance because they suggest that OCD could be considered as a dysfunction of the cognitive ability. Therefore, paying attention to these deficiencies can seriously contribute to the treatment of these patients. Further studies are recommended to further explore the cognitive impairments in patients with OCD.
The current study had some limitations that should be considered. Our participants’ medications may have influenced test performance. The sample size was relatively small in both groups. We used a relatively limited number of cognitive measures that may not have assessed all aspects of the cognitive process, and definitely missed some aspects of cognitive performance that may be impaired in these patients. Future studies should recruit larger samples and employ more comprehensive neuropsychological tests.

Ethical Considerations
Compliance with ethical guidelines

The research was approved by the Local Ethics Committee of Hamadan University of Medical Sciences. Prior to participation in the study, an informed written consent was taken from all participants.

Funding
This article is part of a PhD. thesis of Saeid Yazdi-Ravandi and supported by Hamadan University of Medical Sciences (Grant No: 940125303).

Authors contributions
The authors contributions is as follows: Designed the study and supervised the project: Ali Ghaleiha and Mohammad Ahmadpanah; Designed the study, gathered the data, performed statistical analysis and interpreted the data and drafting the manuscript: Saeid Yazdi-Ravandi; Reviewed and edited the manuscript: Farshid Shamsaei, Nasrin Matinnia, Abbas Moghimbeigi and Jamal Shams; and all authors have seen and approved the final Manuscript.

Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgements
The authors gratefully acknowledge the financial support provided by Vice Chancellor of Research and Technology of Hamadan University of Medical Sciences. 




References
  1. Abramovitch, A., Abramowitz, J. S., & Mittelman, A. (2013). The neuropsychology of adult obsessive–compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(8), 1163-71. [DOI:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.09.004]
  2. Abramovitch, A., Dar, R., Schweiger, A., & Hermesh, H. (2011). Neuropsychological impairments and their association with obsessive-compulsive symptom severity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 26(4), 364-76. [DOI:10.1093/arclin/acr022]
  3. American Psychiatric Publication. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington: American Psychiatric Publication. [DOI:10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596]
  4. Aydın, P. C., Koybasi, G. P., Sert, E., Mete, L., & Oyekcin, D. G. (2014). Executive functions and memory in autogenous and reactive subtype of obsessive-compulsive disorder patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55(4), 904-11. [DOI:10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.01.005]
  5. Balanzá Martínez, V., Selva, G., Martínez Arán, A., Prickaerts, J., Salazar, J., González Pinto, A., et al. (2010). Neurocognition in bipolar disorders-a closer look at comorbidities and medications. European Journal of Pharmacology, 626(1), 87-96. [DOI:10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.10.018]
  6. Bédard, M. J., Joyal, C. C., Godbout, L., & Chantal, S. (2009). Executive functions and the obsessive-compulsive disorder: On the importance of subclinical symptoms and other concomitant factors. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24(6), 585-98. [DOI:10.1093/arclin/acp052]
  7. Benzina, N., Mallet, L., Burguière, E., N’diaye, K., & Pelissolo, A. (2016). Cognitive dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 18(9), 1-11. [DOI:10.1007/s11920-016-0720-3]
  8. Chamberlain, S. R., Blackwell, A. D., Fineberg, N. A., Robbins, T. W., & Sahakian, B. J. (2005). The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: The importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 29(3), 399-419.  [DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.11.006]
  9. Chamberlain, S. R., Fineberg, N. A., Blackwell, A. D., Clark, L., Robbins, T. W., & Sahakian, B. J. (2007). A neuropsychological comparison of obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 654-62.  [DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.07.016]
  10. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
  11. De Geus, F., Denys, D. A., Sitskoorn, M. M., & Westenberg, H. G. (2007). Attention and cognition in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 61(1), 45-53. [DOI:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01609.x]
  12. Deckersbach, T., Savage, C. R., Henin, A., Mataix Cols, D., Otto, M. W., Wilhelm, S., et al. (2000). Reliability and validity of a scoring system for measuring organizational approach in the complex figure test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22(5), 640-8. [DOI:10.1076/1380-3395(200010)22:5;1-9;FT640]
  13. Devinsky, O., Morrell, M. J., & Vogt, B. A. (1995). Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain, 118(1), 279-306. [DOI:10.1093/brain/118.1.279]
  14. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. (1997). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV clinical version (SCID-I/CV). Washington: American Psychiatric Press.
  15. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. (1996). User’s guide for the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I Disorders: SCID-1 clinical version. Washington: American Psychiatric Association.
  16. Foa, E. B., & McNally, R. J. (1986). Sensitivity to feared stimuli in obsessive-compulsives: A dichotic listening analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10(4), 477-85. [DOI:10.1007/BF01173299]
  17. Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., Mazure, C., Fleischmann, R. L., Hill, C. L., et al. (1989). The Yale-Brown obsessive compulsive scale: I. development, use, and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46(11), 1006-11.  [DOI:10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810110048007]
  18. Ghassemzadeh, H., Mojtabai, R., Karamghadiri, N., Noroozian, M., Sharifi, V., & Ebrahimkhani, N. (2012). Neuropsychological and neurological deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: The role of comorbid depression. International Journal of Clinical Medicine, 3(3), 200-10. [DOI:10.4236/ijcm.2012.33040]
  19. Greisberg, S., & McKay, D. (2003). Neuropsychology of obsessive-compulsive disorder: A review and treatment implications. Clinical Psychology Review, 23(1), 95-117. [DOI:10.1016/S0272-7358(02)00232-5]
  20. Greve, K. W., Ingram, F., & Bianchini, K. J. (1998). Latent structure of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in a clinical sample. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13(7), 597-609. [DOI:10.1016/S0887-6177(97)00075-9]
  21. Greve, K. W., Williams, M. C., Haas, W. G., Littell, R. R., & Reinoso, C. (1996). The role of attention in Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11(3), 215-22. [DOI:10.1093/arclin/11.3.215]
  22. Keefe, R. S. (1995). The contribution of neuropsychology to psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152(1), 6-15. [DOI:10.1176/ajp.152.1.6]
  23. Lacerda, A. L., Dalgalarrondo, P., Caetano, D., Haas, G. L., Camargo, E. E., & Keshavan, M. S. (2003). Neuropsychological performance and regional cerebral blood flow in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 27(4), 657-65. [DOI:10.1016/S0278-5846(03)00076-9]
  24. Lavy, E., Van Oppen, P., & Van Den Hout, M. (1994). Selective processing of emotional information in obsessive compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(2), 243-6. [DOI:10.1016/0005-7967(94)90118-X.]
  25. Lewin, A. B., Larson, M. J., Park, J. M., McGuire, J. F., Murphy, T. K., & Storch, E. A. (2014). Neuropsychological functioning in youth with obsessive compulsive disorder: An examination of executive function and memory impairment. Psychiatry Research, 216(1), 108-15. [DOI:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.014]
  26. Macleod, D., & Prior, M. (1996). Attention deficits in adolescents with ADHD and other clinical groups. Child Neuropsychology, 2(1), 1-10. [DOI:10.1080/09297049608401345]
  27. Menzies, L., Chamberlain, S. R., Laird, A. R., Thelen, S. M., Sahakian, B. J., & Bullmore, E. T. (2008). Integrating evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder: The orbitofronto-striatal model revisited. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(3), 525-49. [DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.09.005]
  28. Morein Zamir, S., Papmeyer, M., Durieux, A., Fineberg, N. A., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2013). Investigation of attentional bias in obsessive compulsive disorder with and without depression in visual search. PloS One, 8(11), e80118. [DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0080118]
  29. Moritz, S., Birkner, C., Kloss, M., Jahn, H., Hand, I., Haasen, C., et al. (2002). Executive functioning in obsessive–compulsive disorder, unipolar depression, and schizophrenia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17(5), 477-483. [DOI:10.1016/S0887-6177(01)00130-5]
  30. Moritz, S., Kloss, M., & Jelinek, L. (2010). Negative priming (cognitive inhibition) in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD). Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41(1), 1-5.  [DOI:10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.08.004]
  31. Naderi, N. (1994). [Study of information processing and sore of neuropsychological function in obsessive compulsive patients (Persian)] [MSc. thesis]. Tehran: Iran University of Medical Sciences.
  32. Nedeljkovic, M., Kyrios, M., Moulding, R., Doron, G., Wainwright, K., et al. (2009). Differences in neuropsychological performance between subtypes of obsessive–compulsive disorder. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 43(3), 216-26. [DOI:10.1080/00048670802653273]
  33. Rao, N. P., Reddy, Y. J., Kumar, K. J., Kandavel, T., & Chandrashekar, C. (2008). Are neuropsychological deficits trait markers in OCD? Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 32(6), 1574-9. [DOI:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2008.05.026]
  34. Penadés, R., Catalán, R., Andrés, S., Salamero, M., & Gastó, C. (2005). Executive function and nonverbal memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 133(1), 81-90. [DOI:10.1016/j.psychres.2004.09.005]
  35. Rachman, S. (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(9), 793-802. [DOI:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00040-5]
  36. Rajezi Esfahani, S., Motaghipour, Y., Kamkari, K., Zahiredin, A., & Janbozorgi, M. (2012). Reliability and validity of the persian version of the Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive scale (Y-BOCS). Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, 17(4), 297-303.
  37. Remijnse, P. L., Nielen, M. M., van Balkom, A. J., Cath, D. C., van Oppen, P., Uylings, H. B., et al. (2006). Reduced orbitofrontal-striatal activity on a reversal learning task in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(11), 1225-36. [DOI:10.1001/archpsyc.63.11.1225]
  38. Rotge, J. Y., Guehl, D., Dilharreguy, B., Cuny, E., Tignol, J., Bioulac, B., et al. (2008). Provocation of obsessive-compulsive symptoms: A quantitative voxel-based meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 33(5), 405-12. [PMID] [PMCID]
  39. Saxena, S., & Rauch, S. L. (2000). Functional neuroimaging and the neuroanatomy of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 23(3), 563-86. [DOI:10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70181-7]
  40. Saremi, A. A., Shariat, S. V., Nazari, M. A., & Dolatshahi, B. (2017). Neuropsychological functioning in obsessive-compulsive washers: Drug-naive without depressive symptoms. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience, 8(3), 233-48. [DOI:10.18869/nirp.bcn.8.3.233]
  41. Sharifi, V., Assadi, S. M., Mohammadi, M. R., Amini, H., Kaviani, H., Semnani, Y., et al. (2009). A Persian translation of the structured clinical interview for diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: Psychometric properties. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 50(1), 86-91. [DOI:10.1016/j.comppsych.2008.04.004]
  42. Sherman, E. M., Strauss, E., & Spellacy, F. (1997). Validity of the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) in adults referred for neuropsychological assessment after head injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 11(1), 34-45. [DOI:10.1080/13854049708407027]
  43. Shin, N., Lee, T., Kim, E., & Kwon, J. (2014). Cognitive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44(6), 1121-30. [DOI:10.1017/S0033291713001803]
  44. Spreen, O. , & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychologial tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  45. Snyder, H. R., Kaiser, R. H., Warren, S. L., & Heller, W. (2015). Obsessive-compulsive disorder is associated with broad impairments in executive function: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(2), 301-30. [DOI:10.1177/2167702614534210]
  46. Tanguay, P. E. (2000). Pervasive developmental disorders: A 10-year review. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(9), 1079-95. [DOI:10.1097/00004583-200009000-00007]
  47. Tata, P. R., Leibowitz, J. A., Prunty, M. J., Cameron, M., & Pickering, A. D. (1996). Attentional bias in obsessional compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(1), 53-60. [DOI:10.1016/0005-7967(95)00041-U]
  48. Tükel, R., Gürvit, H., Ertekin, B. A., Oflaz, S., Ertekin, E., Baran, B., et al. (2012). Neuropsychological function in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 53(2), 167-75. [DOI:10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.03.007]
  49. Van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies. Physiology & Behavior, 77(4), 477-82. [DOI:10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00930-7]
  50. Viswanath, B., Janardhan Reddy, Y. C., Kumar, K. J., Kandavel, T., & Chandrashekar, C. R. (2009). Cognitive endophenotypes in OCD: A study of unaffected siblings of probands with familial OCD. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 33(4), 610-5. [DOI:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.02.018]
Type of Study: Original | Subject: Cognitive Neuroscience
Received: 2017/10/10 | Accepted: 2017/12/8 | Published: 2018/11/1

References
1. Abramovitch, A., Abramowitz, J. S., & Mittelman, A. (2013). The neuropsychology of adult obsessive–compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(8), 1163-71. [DOI:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.09.004] [DOI:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.09.004]
2. Abramovitch, A., Dar, R., Schweiger, A., & Hermesh, H. (2011). Neuropsychological impairments and their association with obsessive-compulsive symptom severity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 26(4), 364-76. [DOI:10.1093/arclin/acr022] [DOI:10.1093/arclin/acr022]
3. American Psychiatric Publication. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington: American Psychiatric Publication. [DOI:10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596] [DOI:10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596]
4. Aydın, P. C., Koybasi, G. P., Sert, E., Mete, L., & Oyekcin, D. G. (2014). Executive functions and memory in autogenous and reactive subtype of obsessive-compulsive disorder patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55(4), 904-11. [DOI:10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.01.005] [DOI:10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.01.005]
5. Balanzá Martínez, V., Selva, G., Martínez Arán, A., Prickaerts, J., Salazar, J., González Pinto, A., et al. (2010). Neurocognition in bipolar disorders-a closer look at comorbidities and medications. European Journal of Pharmacology, 626(1), 87-96. [DOI:10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.10.018] [DOI:10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.10.018]
6. Bédard, M. J., Joyal, C. C., Godbout, L., & Chantal, S. (2009). Executive functions and the obsessive-compulsive disorder: On the importance of subclinical symptoms and other concomitant factors. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24(6), 585-98. [DOI:10.1093/arclin/acp052] [DOI:10.1093/arclin/acp052]
7. Benzina, N., Mallet, L., Burguière, E., N'diaye, K., & Pelissolo, A. (2016). Cognitive dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 18(9), 1-11. [DOI:10.1007/s11920-016-0720-3] [DOI:10.1007/s11920-016-0720-3]
8. Chamberlain, S. R., Blackwell, A. D., Fineberg, N. A., Robbins, T. W., & Sahakian, B. J. (2005). The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: The importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 29(3), 399-419. [DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.11.006] [DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.11.006]
9. Chamberlain, S. R., Fineberg, N. A., Blackwell, A. D., Clark, L., Robbins, T. W., & Sahakian, B. J. (2007). A neuropsychological comparison of obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 654-62. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.07.016] [DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.07.016]
10. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. [DOI:10.1002/bs.3830330104]
11. De Geus, F., Denys, D. A., Sitskoorn, M. M., & Westenberg, H. G. (2007). Attention and cognition in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 61(1), 45-53. [DOI:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01609.x] [DOI:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01609.x]
12. Deckersbach, T., Savage, C. R., Henin, A., Mataix Cols, D., Otto, M. W., Wilhelm, S., et al. (2000). Reliability and validity of a scoring system for measuring organizational approach in the complex figure test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22(5), 640-8. [DOI:10.1076/1380-3395(200010)22:5;1-9;FT640] [DOI:10.1076/1380-3395(200010)22:5;1-9;FT640]
13. Devinsky, O., Morrell, M. J., & Vogt, B. A. (1995). Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain, 118(1), 279-306. [DOI:10.1093/brain/118.1.279] [DOI:10.1093/brain/118.1.279]
14. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. (1997). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV clinical version (SCID-I/CV). Washington: American Psychiatric Press.
15. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. (1996). User's guide for the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I Disorders: SCID-1 clinical version. Washington: American Psychiatric Association. [PMCID]
16. Foa, E. B., & McNally, R. J. (1986). Sensitivity to feared stimuli in obsessive-compulsives: A dichotic listening analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10(4), 477-85. [DOI:10.1007/BF01173299] [DOI:10.1007/BF01173299]
17. Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., Mazure, C., Fleischmann, R. L., Hill, C. L., et al. (1989). The Yale-Brown obsessive compulsive scale: I. development, use, and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46(11), 1006-11. [DOI:10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810110048007] [DOI:10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810110048007]
18. Ghassemzadeh, H., Mojtabai, R., Karamghadiri, N., Noroozian, M., Sharifi, V., & Ebrahimkhani, N. (2012). Neuropsychological and neurological deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: The role of comorbid depression. International Journal of Clinical Medicine, 3(3), 200-10. [DOI:10.4236/ijcm.2012.33040] [DOI:10.4236/ijcm.2012.33040]
19. Greisberg, S., & McKay, D. (2003). Neuropsychology of obsessive-compulsive disorder: A review and treatment implications. Clinical Psychology Review, 23(1), 95-117. [DOI:10.1016/S0272-7358(02)00232-5] [DOI:10.1016/S0272-7358(02)00232-5]
20. Greve, K. W., Ingram, F., & Bianchini, K. J. (1998). Latent structure of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in a clinical sample. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13(7), 597-609. [DOI:10.1016/S0887-6177(97)00075-9] [DOI:10.1016/S0887-6177(97)00075-9]
21. Greve, K. W., Williams, M. C., Haas, W. G., Littell, R. R., & Reinoso, C. (1996). The role of attention in Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11(3), 215-22. [DOI:10.1093/arclin/11.3.215] [DOI:10.1093/arclin/11.3.215]
22. Keefe, R. S. (1995). The contribution of neuropsychology to psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152(1), 6-15. [DOI:10.1176/ajp.152.1.6] [DOI:10.1176/ajp.152.1.6]
23. Lacerda, A. L., Dalgalarrondo, P., Caetano, D., Haas, G. L., Camargo, E. E., & Keshavan, M. S. (2003). Neuropsychological performance and regional cerebral blood flow in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 27(4), 657-65. [DOI:10.1016/S0278-5846(03)00076-9] [DOI:10.1016/S0278-5846(03)00076-9]
24. Lavy, E., Van Oppen, P., & Van Den Hout, M. (1994). Selective processing of emotional information in obsessive compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(2), 243-6. [DOI:10.1016/0005-7967(94)90118-X.] [DOI:10.1016/0005-7967(94)90118-X]
25. Lewin, A. B., Larson, M. J., Park, J. M., McGuire, J. F., Murphy, T. K., & Storch, E. A. (2014). Neuropsychological functioning in youth with obsessive compulsive disorder: An examination of executive function and memory impairment. Psychiatry Research, 216(1), 108-15. [DOI:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.014] [DOI:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.014]
26. Macleod, D., & Prior, M. (1996). Attention deficits in adolescents with ADHD and other clinical groups. Child Neuropsychology, 2(1), 1-10. [DOI:10.1080/09297049608401345] [DOI:10.1080/09297049608401345]
27. Menzies, L., Chamberlain, S. R., Laird, A. R., Thelen, S. M., Sahakian, B. J., & Bullmore, E. T. (2008). Integrating evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder: The orbitofronto-striatal model revisited. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(3), 525-49. [DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.09.005] [DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.09.005]
28. Morein Zamir, S., Papmeyer, M., Durieux, A., Fineberg, N. A., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2013). Investigation of attentional bias in obsessive compulsive disorder with and without depression in visual search. PloS One, 8(11), e80118. [DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0080118] [DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0080118]
29. Moritz, S., Birkner, C., Kloss, M., Jahn, H., Hand, I., Haasen, C., et al. (2002). Executive functioning in obsessive–compulsive disorder, unipolar depression, and schizophrenia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17(5), 477-483. [DOI:10.1016/S0887-6177(01)00130-5] [DOI:10.1016/S0887-6177(01)00130-5]
30. Moritz, S., Kloss, M., & Jelinek, L. (2010). Negative priming (cognitive inhibition) in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD). Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41(1), 1-5. [DOI:10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.08.004] [DOI:10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.08.004]
31. Naderi, N. (1994). [Study of information processing and sore of neuropsychological function in obsessive compulsive patients (Persian)] [MSc. thesis]. Tehran: Iran University of Medical Sciences.
32. Nedeljkovic, M., Kyrios, M., Moulding, R., Doron, G., Wainwright, K., et al. (2009). Differences in neuropsychological performance between subtypes of obsessive–compulsive disorder. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 43(3), 216-26. [DOI:10.1080/00048670802653273] [DOI:10.1080/00048670802653273]
33. Rao, N. P., Reddy, Y. J., Kumar, K. J., Kandavel, T., & Chandrashekar, C. (2008). Are neuropsychological deficits trait markers in OCD? Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 32(6), 1574-9. [DOI:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2008.05.026] [DOI:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2008.05.026]
34. Penadés, R., Catalán, R., Andrés, S., Salamero, M., & Gastó, C. (2005). Executive function and nonverbal memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 133(1), 81-90. [DOI:10.1016/j.psychres.2004.09.005] [DOI:10.1016/j.psychres.2004.09.005]
35. Rachman, S. (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(9), 793-802. [DOI:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00040-5] [DOI:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00040-5]
36. Rajezi Esfahani, S., Motaghipour, Y., Kamkari, K., Zahiredin, A., & Janbozorgi, M. (2012). Reliability and validity of the persian version of the Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive scale (Y-BOCS). Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, 17(4), 297-303.
37. Remijnse, P. L., Nielen, M. M., van Balkom, A. J., Cath, D. C., van Oppen, P., Uylings, H. B., et al. (2006). Reduced orbitofrontal-striatal activity on a reversal learning task in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(11), 1225-36. [DOI:10.1001/archpsyc.63.11.1225] [DOI:10.1001/archpsyc.63.11.1225]
38. Rotge, J. Y., Guehl, D., Dilharreguy, B., Cuny, E., Tignol, J., Bioulac, B., et al. (2008). Provocation of obsessive-compulsive symptoms: A quantitative voxel-based meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 33(5), 405-12. [PMID] [PMCID] [PMID] [PMCID]
39. Saxena, S., & Rauch, S. L. (2000). Functional neuroimaging and the neuroanatomy of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 23(3), 563-86. [DOI:10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70181-7] [DOI:10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70181-7]
40. Saremi, A. A., Shariat, S. V., Nazari, M. A., & Dolatshahi, B. (2017). Neuropsychological functioning in obsessive-compulsive washers: Drug-naive without depressive symptoms. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience, 8(3), 233-48. [DOI:10.18869/nirp.bcn.8.3.233] [DOI:10.18869/nirp.bcn.8.3.233]
41. Sharifi, V., Assadi, S. M., Mohammadi, M. R., Amini, H., Kaviani, H., Semnani, Y., et al. (2009). A Persian translation of the structured clinical interview for diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: Psychometric properties. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 50(1), 86-91. [DOI:10.1016/j.comppsych.2008.04.004] [DOI:10.1016/j.comppsych.2008.04.004]
42. Sherman, E. M., Strauss, E., & Spellacy, F. (1997). Validity of the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) in adults referred for neuropsychological assessment after head injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 11(1), 34-45. [DOI:10.1080/13854049708407027] [DOI:10.1080/13854049708407027]
43. Shin, N., Lee, T., Kim, E., & Kwon, J. (2014). Cognitive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44(6), 1121-30. [DOI:10.1017/S0033291713001803] [DOI:10.1017/S0033291713001803]
44. Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychologial tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
45. Snyder, H. R., Kaiser, R. H., Warren, S. L., & Heller, W. (2015). Obsessive-compulsive disorder is associated with broad impairments in executive function: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(2), 301-30. [DOI:10.1177/2167702614534210] [DOI:10.1177/2167702614534210]
46. Tanguay, P. E. (2000). Pervasive developmental disorders: A 10-year review. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(9), 1079-95. [DOI:10.1097/00004583-200009000-00007] [DOI:10.1097/00004583-200009000-00007]
47. Tata, P. R., Leibowitz, J. A., Prunty, M. J., Cameron, M., & Pickering, A. D. (1996). Attentional bias in obsessional compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(1), 53-60. [DOI:10.1016/0005-7967(95)00041-U] [DOI:10.1016/0005-7967(95)00041-U]
48. Tükel, R., Gürvit, H., Ertekin, B. A., Oflaz, S., Ertekin, E., Baran, B., et al. (2012). Neuropsychological function in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 53(2), 167-75. [DOI:10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.03.007] [DOI:10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.03.007]
49. Van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies. Physiology & Behavior, 77(4), 477-82. [DOI:10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00930-7] [DOI:10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00930-7]
50. Viswanath, B., Janardhan Reddy, Y. C., Kumar, K. J., Kandavel, T., & Chandrashekar, C. R. (2009). Cognitive endophenotypes in OCD: A study of unaffected siblings of probands with familial OCD. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 33(4), 610-5. [DOI:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.02.018] [DOI:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.02.018]

Add your comments about this article : Your username or Email:
CAPTCHA

Send email to the article author


Rights and permissions
Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

© 2024 CC BY-NC 4.0 | Basic and Clinical Neuroscience

Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb