Volume 10, Issue 5 (September & October 2019)                   BCN 2019, 10(5): 475-484 | Back to browse issues page


XML Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Rezaei M, Azizian F. Neuropsychological Decomposing Stroop Interference Into Different Cognitive Monitoring: An Exploratory Factor Analysis. BCN 2019; 10 (5) :475-484
URL: http://bcn.iums.ac.ir/article-1-1012-en.html
1- Departments of Clinical Psychology, Shahid Beheshti Hospital, Zanjan University of Medical Sciences, Zanjan, Iran.
2- Department of Psychiatry, Imam Hossein Hospital, Alborz University of Medical Sciences, Karaj, Iran.
Full-Text [PDF 659 kb]       |   Abstract (HTML) 
Full-Text:  
1. Introduction
What is required in a conflictual situation such as overcoming a habitual action in favor of an unusual one? It takes mind to involve at least 3 cognitive operations; to detect interference between 2 parallel processes, to make a decision (decide to focus on the information related to the goal and at the same time to inhibit the unrelated information), and to inhibit habitual action (to form an output code based on the appropriate decision). These complex mental operations refer to “interference” resolution. Posner et al. (Petersen & Posner, 2012; Posner, 2008, 2012a, 2012b; Posner & Dehaene, 1994; Posner & Petersen, 1990; Posner, Rothbart, Sheese, & Tang, 2007) ascribe such an operation to executive network of attention (see Petersen & Posner, 2012 for reviewing this and two other attentional networks). The executive attention network detects error and resolves interference among contradictory parallel responses (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001; Petersen & Posner, 2012).
Various cognitive tasks measure interference between stimulus dimensions such as temporal dimensions (Nazari, Mirloo, Rezaei, & Soltanlou, 2018; Nazari et al., 2013) and spatial features (Simon & Berbaum, 1990; Simon, Sly, & Vilapakkam, 1981). The most widely studied is the classic Stroop interference between a word name and its ink color (MacLeod, 1991). The task was originally developed by Stroop. In Stroop task, the subjects see words that denote colors (red, green, blue, and yellow) printed in a corresponding color (e.g. the word red is written in red ink) or in a non-corresponding color (e.g. the word blue is written in red ink) and the subjects are required by instruction to name the ink color while inhibiting the meaning of the word. When the color and the word are congruent, the task is easy; when the color and the word are incongruent, people experience interference (MacLeod, 1991; Stroop, 1935).
The Stroop interference indicates that Reaction Time (RT) is consistently longer in the incongruent trials compared with the congruent ones (see MacLeod, 1991, for a review). The interference occurs among the automatic process of word reading and the effortful process of color naming (Augustinova & Ferrand, 2014; Cohen, Dunbar, & McClelland, 1990; Luo, 1999; Schneider & Chein, 2003). Thus, the so-called Stroop-interference denotes the ability to inhibit a usual response (i.e. an overlearned response) in favor of an unusual one (Homack & Riccio, 2004).
The variety of Stroop test versions is one of the challenges of the literature. Many later studies (Chen, Wong, Chen, & Au, 2000; Davidson & Wright, 2002; Farhadian, Akbarfahimi, Hassani Abharian, Hosseini, & Shokri, 2017; Hekmat, Alam Mehrjerdi, Moradi, Ekhtiari, & Bakhshi, 2011; Hinkin, Castellon, Hardy, Granholm, & Siegle, 1999; Rezaei, Ashayeri, Yazdandoost, & Asgharnejad, 2003; Saremi, Shariat, Nazari, & Dolatshahi, 2017) have implemented computerized stimuli that facilitate the accurate measurement of RT (Homack & Riccio, 2004). There are two types of RT measurements; the RT for the verbal response and the RT for the motor response (pressing the button). Most studies have used the first type.
In recent years, questions have been raised about the nature of processing interference. Lupker and Katz claimed that Stroop interference could occur at 4 possible stages or processes: 1. an input process; 2. a decision process; 3. a response selection process; and 4. a response output process. Two alternative explanations of the Stroop phenomenon correspond to the stages of information processing proposed by Lupker and Katz.
Several researchers argued that the difference in performance on congruent and incongruent trials could arise from response interference (Cohen, Dunbar, & McClelland, 1990; Duncan-Johnson & Kopell, 1981; MacLeod, 1991; Roelofs, 2003). Lupker and Katz claimed that in the response selection process, an output code must be formed based on the appropriate decision (e.g. a phonetic representation in a naming task, a motor code representation in a button-pressing task, and so on). This means that the presentation of the irrelevant word automatically activates the responses with similar meanings that facilitate response selection on congruent trials but interferes with response selection on incongruent trials (De Houwer, 2003).
Several authors (Brown & Besner, 2001; Davelaar & Besner, 1988; Green, Locker, Boyer, & Sturz, 2016; Hasshim & Parris, 2015; Klopfer, 1996; Luo, 1999; Seymour, 1977; White, Risko, & Besner, 2016; Williams, Mathews, & MacLeod, 1996) have claimed that Stroop interference might occur at earlier processing stages related to semantic or conceptual encoding. Seymour debated that Stroop interference occurred because the to-be-named color and the to-be-ignored word activated two similar semantic codes.
Many analysts (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001; Van Veen, Cohen, Botvinick, Stenger, & Carter, 2001) have claimed that the information is processed at different levels. As Van Veen et al. argued, “Theoretically, interference might occur at any or all of these levels” (Van Veen et al., 2001). There was widespread behavioral (De Houwer, 2003; Schmidt & Cheesman, 2005; Zhang & Kornblum, 1998; Zhang, Zhang, & Kornblum, 1999) and neuropsychological (Chen, Bailey, Tiernan, & West, 2011; Chen, Lei, Ding, Li, & Chen, 2013; Liu, Banich, Jacobson, & Tanabe, 2004; Melcher & Gruber, 2009; Taylor, Kornblum, Lauber, Minoshima, & Koeppe, 1997; Van Veen & Carter, 2005; Zysset, Muller, Lohmann, & von Cramon, 2001) support to give an account that both forms of interference contribute to the Stroop interference-effect.
The present study aims to determine the number and nature of the factors necessary to account for the multiple components of Stroop interference. We conducted an exploratory factor analysis to claim that we did not make a priori assumption about the nature of interference. 
2. Methods
2.1. Study Participants 

Zanjan University has four faculties: agriculture, Humanities, Science, and Engineering. In each of the faculties, an announcement was issued to recruit volunteer students to participate in this study. The sample consisted of 247 undergraduate and postgraduate students (Table 1).


2.2 Measure
We employed a computerized version of the Stroop task adapted to the Iranian population. The Persian version was developed by Ravan Tajhiz Sina Company. The software used the set of 4 stimuli defined by color words written in Persian alphabet; “ghermez” (Persian word for “red”), “sabz” (Persian word for “green”), “zard” (Persian word for “yellow”), and “Ābi” (Persian word for “blue”) that were presented in red, green, yellow, and blue ink. All 48 congruent stimuli were the color words shown in the corresponding ink (e.g. the word “blue” was printed in blue ink), while 48 incongruent stimuli were the color words presented in the different ink (e.g. the word “red” was written in blue ink). The output was as follows: 1. RT under congruent and incongruent conditions: interval between the perception of the color of word and pressing the colored button; 2. Omission error under congruent and incongruent conditions: the failure to respond to target button; and 3. Commission error under congruent and incongruent conditions: responses are given to non-targets button (e.g. the subject press the “red” button when the “blue” ink is presented). 
Interference RT was calculated as the difference between the incongruent and congruent mean reaction times. The internal consistency of RTs was 0.6, 0.83, and 0.97 in 3 stages, respectively, by Ghadiri, Jazayeri, Ashayeri, and Ghazi-Tabatabaei. Internal consistency for error numbers were also 0.55, 0.78, and 0.79 in 3 stages, respectively. Several studies (Ghadiri et al., 2006; Hekmat et al., 2011; Rezaei et al., 2003; Saremi et al., 2017) have used the Iranian version of computerized Stroop task to study neuropsychological substrates of psychiatric disorders.
2.3. Study procedure
All 96 trials were randomly presented at the center of a 15-inch computer screen. The display time was 2s, and the inter-stimulus interval was 800 ms. The response buttons were labeled with color patches on the keyboard. The subjects were instructed to press the same color button as the word ink color.
2.4. Data analysis
The analysis of the Stroop interference was based on the assumption that there was an observed interference as measured by the task we used. Thus, we used a related t-test to examine whether the incongruent mean RT is significantly different from the congruent mean RT. Moreover, an exploratory principal components analysis was conducted on the correlations of 6 variables (Table 2).


The Kaiser eigenvalue (Kaiser, 1960) criterion (i.e. eigenvalues >1.0) was considered for determining the number of factors for extraction. The orthogonal rotation of the factors by the varimax method yielded the factor structure given in Table 3.
 





3. Results
The incongruent mean RT (Mean±SD=959±175) and congruent mean RT (Mean±SD=902±161) were significantly different (t [246]=23, two-tailed P=0.001). Thus, there is Stroop interference. Two factors were initially extracted. The first factor accounted for 56% of the variance, and the second factor accounted for 23%. The first factor seems to be semantic interference and the second one is response interference. Communalities (i.e. percentage of indicator variance accounted for by the solution) ranged from 0.75 (congruent omission) to 0.82 (congruent commission).
4. Discussion
In the present study, we used a manual response (button press) format of the computerized Stroop task to explore the components of Stroop interfere. We found that RT was significantly longer in the incongruent condition compared with the congruent condition, indicating that the task elicited the Stroop interference. Furthermore, incongruent trials showed significantly higher commission and omission errors than congruent trials, demonstrating the task-induced Stroop interference. The present findings are consistent with other studies, which found the Stroop interference in manual response task (Ila & Polich, 1999; Schmidt & Cheesman, 2005; Sharma & McKenna, 1998; Sugg & McDonald, 1994). This finding corroborates the idea of speed-accuracy tradeoff (Fan, Flombaum, McCandliss, Thomas, & Posner, 2003; Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz, & Posner, 2002; MacLeod, 1991; Stroop, 1935). It has been suggested that incongruent conditions result in longer RTs and higher error rates than congruent conditions and vice versa. As a consequence of observing Stroop interference, we achieved our purpose of conducting an exploratory factor analysis. The analysis yielded two factors.
Increased RT (both congruent and incongruent), together with omission error increasing (both congruent and incongruent) were loaded on factor one. It seems that the common features of this factor are compatible with semantic interference. Increased RT primarily reflects semantic processing, i.e. whether the concept represented by color word and the concept represented by color ink are incongruent. It seems reasonable to assume that these effortful trials indicate the subject’s ability to detect semantic interference. The adherents of semantic explanation believe that interference occurs at the earlier processing related to semantic encoding before response output. As we learned from Seymour, the to-be-named color dimension of the word and the to-be-ignored word trigger two similar semantic codes. In a nutshell, the primary explanation for the semantic competition is that Stroop stimuli trigger semantic representations of both color and word dimensions; thus, the semantic competition between these two dimensions is created before the response output (Green et al., 2016). Luo explained that color naming required the activation of an appropriate network in the verbal-lexical system. But, word reading requires only the activation of the relevant network in the lexical-verbal system, and, in this case, the operation of the semantic system is optional. The main cognitive challenge for the participants in our manual task and other versions is “to ignore or override” the semantic codes of the verbal-lexical system for responding. If the person fails to override, he or she will either respond long or will not select any response. Botvinick et al. called the type of cognitive monitoring to inhibit the verbal-lexical system (word reading) as response override (Botvinick et al., 2001).
The second finding was both congruent and incongruent commission error loaded on factor two that was called response interference. As we described, commission errors are made when responses are given to the non-targets button (e.g. the subject presses the “red” button when the “blue” ink is presented). According to Lupker and Katz, these errors can occur in the third (a response selection process) and fourth stages (a response output process). In the response selection process, an output code must be formed based on the appropriate decision (a motor code representation for the button-pressing task; e.g. the subject plan presses the “red” button when the “red” ink is presented, and to inhibit other button press).
Returning to the question posed at the beginning of this study, it is now possible to state that the findings of this study are consistent with the multiple-stage account (Chen et al., 2011; Chen et al., 2013; De Houwer, 2003; Liu et al., 2004; Melcher & Gruber, 2009; Schmidt & Cheesman, 2005; Taylor et al., 1997; Van Veen & Carter, 2005; Zhang & Kornblum, 1998; Zhang et al., 1999; Zysset et al., 2001) claiming that the Stroop interference is because of both semantic and response interferences. These studies used many modified versions of the Stroop color-word task, by which they could separate semantic interference from response interference (for example, see De Houwer, 2003 for such a modified version of the Stroop task). Van Veen and Carter used a modified version of the Stroop task, by which they could separate semantic from response interference through fMRI data. They identified that both semantic and response interference elicited independent activation in anterior cingulate and prefrontal and parietal brain regions. They concluded that the brain had discrete and parallel attentional processes for resolving these various conflicts. Other neuroimaging studies have also demonstrated two independent neural networks underlying semantic and response interferences in the Stroop task (Chen et al., 2011; Chen et al., 2013; Liu et al., 2004; Melcher & Gruber, 2009; Taylor et al., 1997; Van Veen & Carter, 2005; Zysset et al., 2001).
Neuroimaging studies (Chen et al., 2013; Egner & Hirsch, 2005; Liu et al., 2004; Ruff, Woodward, Laurens, & Liddle, 2001; Van Veen & Carter, 2005; Van Veen et al., 2001; Zysset et al., 2001) have shown that the activation of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in Stroop and Stroop-like tasks require people to override either a prepotent response or a rather strong conflicting dimension. These results propose the detection of interference for the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex as a definite executive function that occurs at response-related levels of processing.
It appears that the findings of the current study are limited by the use of a manual version of the Stroop task owing to its apparent motor component. Further exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses should be done to establish whether the vocal tasks support our findings, including two types of interference.
Ethical Considerations
Compliance with ethical guidelines

All ethical principles were considered in this article. The participants were informed about the purpose of the research and its implementation stages; they were also assured about the confidentiality of their information; Moreover, They were allowed to leave the study whenever they wish, and if desired, the results of the research would be available to them.
Funding
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Authors' contributions
All authors contributed in preparing this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest. 


References
  1. Augustinova, M., & Ferrand, L. (2014). Automaticity of word reading: Evidence from the semantic stroop paradigm. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(5), 343-8. [DOI:10.1177/0963721414540169]
  2. Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624-52. [DOI:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624] [PMID]
  3. Brown, M., & Besner, D. (2001). On a variant of Stroop’s paradigm: Which cognitions press your buttons? Memory & Cognition, 29(6), 903-4. [DOI:10.3758/BF03196419]
  4. Chen, A., Bailey, K., Tiernan, B. N., & West, R. (2011). Neural correlates of stimulus and response interference in a 2-1 mapping stroop task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 80(2), 129-38. [DOI:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.02.012] [PMID]
  5. Chen, E. Y., Wong, A. W., Chen, R. Y. L., & Au, W. Y. (2000). Stroop interference and facilitation effects in first-episode schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenic Research, 48(1), 29-44. [DOI:10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00107-9]
  6. Chen, Z., Lei, X., Ding, C., Li, H., & Chen, A. (2013). The neural mechanisms of semantic and response conflicts: An fMRI study of practice-related effects in the stroop task. Neuroimage, 66, 577-84. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.028] [PMID]
  7. Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97(3), 332-61. [DOI:10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.332] [PMID]
  8. Davelaar, E., & Besner, D. (1988). Word identification: Imageability, semantics, and the content-functor distinction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40(4), 789-99. [DOI:10.1080/14640748808402299]
  9. Davidson, E. J., & Wright, P. (2002). Selective processing of weight-and shape-related words in bulimia nervosa: Use of a computerised Stroop test. Eating Behaviors, 3(3), 261-73. [DOI:10.1016/S1471-0153(02)00064-8]
  10. De Houwer, J. (2003). On the role of stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus compatibility in the Stroop effect. Memory & Cognition, 31(3), 353-9. [DOI:10.3758/BF03194393]
  11. Duncan-Johnson, C. C., & Kopell, B. S. (1981). The Stroop effect: Brain potentials localize the source of interference. Science, 214(4), 938-40. [DOI:10.1126/science.7302571] [PMID]
  12. Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task. Neuroimage, 24(2), 539-47. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.007] [PMID]
  13. Fan, J., Flombaum, J. I., McCandliss, B. D., Thomas, K. M., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Cognitive and brain consequences of conflict. Neuroimage, 18(1), 42-57. [DOI:10.1006/nimg.2002.1319] [PMID]
  14. Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340-7. [DOI:10.1162/089892902317361886] [PMID]
  15. Farhadian, M., Akbarfahimi, M., Hassani Abharian, P., Hosseini, S. G., & Shokri, S. (2017). Assessment of executive functions in methamphetamineaddicted individuals: Emphasis on duration of addiction and abstinence. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience Journal, 8(2), 147-54. [DOI:10.18869/nirp.bcn.8.2.147] [PMID] [PMCID]
  16. Ghadiri, F., Jazayeri, A., A’shayeri, H., & Ghazi-Tabatabaei, M. (2007). [The role of cognitive rehabilitation in reduction of executive function deficits and obsessive-compulsive symptoms in schizo-obsessive patients (Persian)]. Archives of Rehabilitation, 7(4), 11-24.
  17. Green, M. L., Locker Jr, L., Boyer, T. W., & Sturz, B. R. (2016). Stroop-like interference in a match-to-sample task: Further evidence for semantic competition? Learning and Motivation, 56, 53-64. [DOI:10.1016/j.lmot.2016.09.003]
  18. Hasshim, N., & Parris, B. A. (2015). Assessing stimulus-stimulus (semantic) conflict in the Stroop task using saccadic two-to-one color response mapping and preresponse pupillary measures. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 77(8), 2601-10. [DOI:10.3758/s13414-015-0971-9] [PMID]
  19. Hekmat, S., Alam Mehrjerdi, Z., Moradi, A., Ekhtiari, H., & Bakhshi, S. (2011). Cognitive flexibility, attention and speed of mental processing in opioid and methamphetamine addicts in comparison with non-addicts. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience, 2(2), 12-9.
  20. Hinkin, C. H., Castellon, S. A., Hardy, D. J., Granholm, E., & Siegle, G. (1999). Computerized and traditional stroop task dysfunction in HIV-1 infection. Neuropsychology, 13(2), 306-16. [DOI:10.1037/0894-4105.13.2.306] [PMID]
  21. Homack, S., & Riccio, C. A. (2004). A meta-analysis of the sensitivity and specificity of the Stroop Color and Word Test with children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19(6), 725-43. [DOI:10.1016/j.acn.2003.09.003] [PMID]
  22. Ila, A. B., & Polich, J. (1999). P300 and response time from a manual Stroop task. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110(2), 367-73. [DOI:10.1016/S0168-5597(98)00053-7]
  23. Kaiser, H. F. (1960). The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20(1), 141-51. [DOI:10.1177/001316446002000116]
  24. Klopfer, D. S. (1996). Stroop interference and color-word similarity. Psychological Science, 7(3), 150-7. [DOI:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00348.x]
  25. Liu, X., Banich, M. T., Jacobson, B. L., & Tanabe, J. L. (2004). Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI. Neuroimage, 22(3), 1097-06. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.02.033] [PMID]
  26. Luo, C. R. (1999). Semantic competition as the basis of stroop interference: Evidence from color-word matching tasks. Psychological Science, 10(1), 35-40. [DOI:10.1111/1467-9280.00103]
  27. Lupker, S. J., & Katz, A. N. (1981). Input, decision, and response factors in picture-word interference interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 7(4), 269-82. [DOI:10.1037//0278-7393.7.4.269]
  28. MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop Effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163-203. [DOI:10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163] [PMID]
  29. Melcher, T., & Gruber, O. (2009). Decomposing interference during stroop performance into different conflict factors: An event-related fMRI study. Cortex, 45(2), 189-200. [DOI:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.06.004] [PMID]
  30. Nazari, M. A., Mirloo, M. M., Rezaei, M., & Soltanlou, M. (2018). Emotional stimuli facilitate time perception in children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Neuropsychology, 12(2), 165-75. [DOI:10.1111/jnp.12111] [PMID]
  31. Nazari, M. A., Mirloo, M. M., Soltanlou, M., Rezaee, M., Roshani, A., & Asadzadeh, S. (2013). Design and development ofthe time discrimination threshold computerized task (Persian)]. Advances in Cognitive Science, 15(1), 67-76.
  32. Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73-89. [DOI:10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525] [PMID] [PMCID]
  33. Posner, M. (2012). Attentional networks and consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(64), 1-4. [DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00064] [PMID]
  34. Posner, M. I. (2008). Measuring Alertness. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1129(1), 193-9. [DOI:10.1196/annals.1417.011] [PMID]
  35. Posner, M. I. (2012b). Imaging attention networks. NeuroImage, 61(2), 450-6. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.040] [PMID] [PMCID]
  36. Posner, M. I., & Dehaene, S. (1994). Attentional networks. Trends in Neurosciences, 17(2), 75-9. [DOI:10.1016/0166-2236(94)90078-7]
  37. Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25-42. [DOI:10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325] [PMID]
  38. Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Tang, Y. (2007). The anterior cingulate gyrus and the mechanism of self-regulation. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 391-5. [DOI:10.3758/CABN.7.4.391]
  39. Rezaei, M., Ashayeri, H., Yazdandoost, R., & Asgharnejad, A. A. (2003). Frontal lobe cognitive functioning in conduct disordered adolescents (Persian). Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, 9(2), 30-9.
  40. Roelofs, A. (2003). Goal-referenced selection of verbal action: modeling attentional control in the Stroop task. Psychological review, 110(1), 88-125. [DOI:10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.88] [PMID]
  41. Ruff, C. C., Woodward, T. S., Laurens, K. R., & Liddle, P. F. (2001). The role of the anterior cingulate cortex in conflict processing: Evidence from reverse stroop interference. NeuroImage, 14(5), 1150-8. [DOI:10.1006/nimg.2001.0893] [PMID]
  42. Saremi, A. A., Shariat, S. V., Nazari, M. A., & Dolatshahi, B. (2017). Neuropsychological functioning in obsessive-compulsive washers: Drug-naive without depressive symptoms. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience Journal, 8(3), 233-48. [DOI:10.18869/nirp.bcn.8.3.233] [PMID] [PMCID]
  43. Schmidt, J. R., & Cheesman, J. (2005). Dissociating stimulus-stimulus and response-response effects in the Stroop task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(2), 132-8. [DOI:10.1037/h0087468] [PMID]
  44. Schneider, W., & Chein, J. M. (2003). Controlled & automatic processing: Behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms. Cognitive Science, 27(3), 525-59. [DOI:10.1207/s15516709cog2703_8]
  45. Seymour, P. H. (1977). Conceptual encoding and locus of the Stroop effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29(2), 245-65. [DOI:10.1080/14640747708400601] [PMID]
  46. Sharma, D., & McKenna, F. P. (1998). Differential components of the manual and vocal Stroop tasks. Memory & Cognition, 26(5), 1033-40. [DOI:10.3758/BF03201181]
  47. Simon, J. R., & Berbaum, K. (1990). Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: the ‘Stroop effect’vs. the ‘Simon effect’. Acta psychologica, 73(2), 159-70. [DOI:10.1016/0001-6918(90)90077-S]
  48. Simon, J. R., Sly, P. E., & Vilapakkam, S. (1981). Effect of compatibility of S-R mapping on reactions toward the stimulus source. Acta psychologica, 47(1), 63-81. [DOI:10.1016/0001-6918(81)90039-1]
  49. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of experimental psychology, 18(6), 643-62. [DOI:10.1037/h0054651]
  50. Sugg, M. J., & McDonald, J. E. (1994). Time course of inhibition in color-response and word-response versions of the Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 20(3), 647-75. [DOI:10.1037/0096-1523.20.3.647] [PMID]
  51. Taylor, S. F., Kornblum, S., Lauber, E. J., Minoshima, S., & Koeppe, R. A. (1997). Isolation of specific interference processing in the stroop task: PET activation studies. NeuroImage, 6(2), 81-92. [DOI:10.1006/nimg.1997.0285] [PMID]
  52. Van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2005). Separating semantic conflict and response conflict in the Stroop task: A functional MRI study. NeuroImage, 27(3), 497-504. [DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.042] [PMID]
  53. Van Veen, V., Cohen, J. D., Botvinick, M. M., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex, conflict monitoring, and levels of processing. NeuroImage, 14(6), 1302-8. [DOI:10.1006/nimg.2001.0923] [PMID]
  54. White, D., Risko, E. F., & Besner, D. (2016). The semantic Stroop Effect: An ex-Gaussian analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23(5), 1576-81. [DOI:10.3758/s13423-016-1014-9] [PMID]
  55. Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 3-24. [DOI:10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3] [PMID]
  56. Zhang, H. H., Zhang, J., & Kornblum, S. (1999). A parallel distributed processing model of stimulus- stimulus and stimulus-response compatibility. Cognitive Psychology, 38(3), 386-32. [DOI:10.1006/cogp.1998.0703] [PMID]
  57. Zhang, H., & Kornblum, S. (1998). The effects of stimulus-response mapping and irrelevant stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus overlap in four-choice Stroop tasks with single-carrier stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 24(1), 3-19. [DOI:10.1037//0096-1523.24.1.3]
  58. Zysset, S., Muller, K., Lohmann, G., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). Color-word matching stroop task: Separating interference and response conflict. NeuroImage, 13(1), 29-36. [DOI:10.1006/nimg.2000.0665] [PMID]
Type of Study: Original | Subject: Cognitive Neuroscience
Received: 2017/08/27 | Accepted: 2018/09/19 | Published: 2019/09/1

Add your comments about this article : Your username or Email:
CAPTCHA

Send email to the article author


Rights and permissions
Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

© 2024 CC BY-NC 4.0 | Basic and Clinical Neuroscience

Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb