Volume 8, Issue 3 (May & June 2017 -- 2017)                   BCN 2017, 8(3): 193-202 | Back to browse issues page


XML Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Maleki M, Hassanpour-Ezatti M, Navaeian M. Research Paper: Cross State-dependent Learning Interaction Between Scopolamine and Morphine in Mice: The Role of Dorsal Hippocampus. BCN 2017; 8 (3) :193-202
URL: http://bcn.iums.ac.ir/article-1-704-en.html
1- Department of Biology, School of Basic Sciences, Shahed University, Tehran, Iran.
2- Department of Biology, Yadegar-e Imam Khomeini (rah) Shahr-e Rey Branch, Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran.
Full-Text [PDF 673 kb]       |   Abstract (HTML) 
Full-Text:  
1. Introduction
Drug-induced state-dependent learning and memory is defined as experiences in which an animal learns skills or relationships between different stimuli under the influence of a psychoactive drug. Memories, stored under drugs influence, are more likely to be performed, recalled, or retrieved under the 
influence of the same or similar drugs than no or dissimilar drugs (Young, 2014). But, cross state-dependent learning is defined as a situation in which administration of a drug at pretesting periods can restore the amnesia induced by other dissimilar drugs administrated during pre/post-training period and vice versa (Jamali-Raeufy, Nasehi, Ebrahimi-ghiri, & Zarrindast, 2011). 
Scopolamine (SCO) is a cholinergic antagonist that blocks most types of muscarinic receptors and induces amnesia via dysregulation of cholinergic system activity (Piri, Rostampour, Nasehi, & Zarrindast, 2013). Scopolamine is also used by scientists to induce memory failure in a variety of learning and memory models, especially in step-down passive avoidance memory in human and mice (Duka, Edelmann, Schutt, Dorow, & Fichte, 1992; Lo et al., 2014). Its effect on the learning, acquisition, and short-term retention of spatial memory tasks is also evaluated (Klinkenberg & Blokland, 2010). It is proposed that SCO memory impairing effect is mediated via encoding disruption in both CA3 and CA1 sub-regions of the hippocampus (Antonova et al., 2011). Its state-dependent memory effect in human and some laboratory animals was also reported previously (Ghorbanalizadeh et al., 2008; Petersen, 1979). Central effects of scopolamine show reversible properties and some neuromodulatory compounds. For example, galantamine can improve its amnesic effect on memory function (Ramakrishnan, Amatya, DeSaer, Dalhoff, & Eggerichs, 2014). 
Impairment in passive avoidance memory can be also induced by both pre-training and/or pretest administration of morphine (Zarrindast, Ardjmand, Rezayof, & Ahmadi, 2013). This impairment is reversed by the mu–opioid receptor antagonist naloxone, indicating that mu-opioid receptor plays a role in state-dependent learning of morphine (Jafari-Sabet & Jannat-Dastjerdi, 2009). Patti et al. (2005) denoted that such deficits did not result from other Central Nervous System (CNS) side effects of morphine such as change in anxiety levels or locomotor activity. 
On the other hand, an interaction was observed between SCO and morphine effects on different learning and memory tasks such as conditioned place preference in rats (Zhai et al., 2008). The study on genetically manipulated mice showed that muscarinic receptors play important roles in many behavioral effects of morphine (Carrigan & Dykstra, 2007). Moreover, administration of SCO can inhibit parts of morphine withdrawal signs in morphine-dependent rats (Xiang et al., 2006). Although the role of SCO in modulation of stimulation of brain reward system impairment via hippocampus in self-stimulation behavior was less consistent (Vega-Flores, Gruart, & Delgado-García, 2014), it was shown that it can induce recovery in an inhibitory avoidance response after its extinction in rats (Roldán, Cobos-Zapiain, Quirarte, & Prado-Alcalá, 2001)
The dorsal pole of the hippocampus in rodents is known as functional equivalent to the human posterior hippocampus (Fanselow & Dong, 2010). Further, due to its central role in inhibitory avoidance tasks, scientist used direct itra-CA1injection of different compounds in rodents to evaluate different neurotransmitters and their effects upon learning and memory functions (Mahmoodi, Ahmadi, pourmotabbed, Oryan, & Zarrindast, 2010; Zarrindast, Ownegh, Rezayof, & Ownegh, 2014). In this regard, the manipulation of M1 muscarinic receptors on CA1 pyramidal cells proved the participation of dorsal hippocampus muscarinic receptors on learning and memory functions (Leung and Peloquin, 2009). Other researchers also showed the implication of a dorsal hippocampal μ-opioid receptor mechanism in state-dependent memory (Jafari-Sabet & Jannat-Dastjerdi, 2009; Zarrindast, Fazli-Tabaei, Ahmadi, & Yahyavi, 2006a). Moreover, the results of other studies indicate that cholinergic system in this area has critical function in mediating the cross state-dependency of learning and memory (Alijanpour and Rezayof, 2013). Muscarinic receptors have strong modulatory effect on inhibitory synaptic rhythms in hippocampus (Alger, Nagode, & Tang, 2014). Finally, the regulatory role of opioid receptor on muscarinic receptor-mediated synaptic responses in rat hippocampus was reported (Kearns, Morton, Bulters, & Davies, 2001)
Step-down passive avoidance model in rodents is known as a reliable method for pharmacological evaluation of memory and memory-related phenomena such as Long-Term Potentiation (LTP) in CA1 (Whitlock, 2006). The step-down passive avoidance memory in animals relies heavily on the dorsal hippocampus activity (Jafari-Sabet, 2011). Therefore, this method is routinely selected to evaluate hippocampal related state-dependent learning in rodents. The current study aimed at identifying the existence of cross state-dependency of learning between SCO and morphine in step-down type passive avoidance task in mice, considering the role of dorsal hippocampus.
2. Methods
2.1. Animals and housing conditions
Adult male mice (NMRI species), weighing 20 to 25 g at the time of surgery (Shahed University, Tehran, Iran) were used in the current study. Access to food and water was unrestricted and each 4 animals were housed in a cage. Mice 
were housed in an animal facility maintained at 22±2ºC and 55%±5% relative humidity under a 12:12 hour light/dark cycle with lights on at 7:00 AM. Ten animals were used in each experimental group. Each animal was used once only. All procedures were performed in accordance with the institutional guidelines for animal care and use.
2.2. Drugs
Morphine sulfate was purchased from Temad (Tehran, Iran) and was used intraperitoneally (i.p.). Scopolamine hydrochloride and naloxone hydrochloride were purchased from Sigma (St. Louis, MO, USA). Scopolamine hydrobromide was dissolved in sterile saline and injected into CA1 of dorsal hippocampus in a volume of 0.5 μL/site.
2.3. Surgery
Mice were anesthetized with a ketamine (50 mg/kg, i.p.)+xylazine (5 mg/kg, i.p.) mixture. Stereotaxic surgery was performed in a standard rodent stereotaxic procedure. Stainless steel guide cannulae (22-gauge) were bilaterally implanted in the CA1 region according to the rat brain atlas of Paxinos and Watson (Paxinos & Franklin, 2001). Guide cannula insertion positions were determined as follows: AP: –2mm from bregma, L: ±1.6 from the sagittal suture, and V: –1.5 mm from the skull surface. The guide cannulae were fixed to the skull by stainless steel skull screws and dental acrylic. Stainless steel stylets (27-gauge; outer diameter: 0.4 mm) were inserted into the guide cannulae to keep them free of debris. All animals were allowed to recover from surgery and clear anesthetic effects for 1 week. 
2.4. Intra-CA1 injection
During the Intra-CA1 injection, the animals were restrained manually. A 27-gauge injection needle (1 mm below the tip of the guide cannulae) was inserted into the guide cannula. Drugs were delivered manually with a 2.5 μL Hamilton microsyringe attached to the injection cannula via polyethylene tubing (PE-10). The total injection volume was 1 µL per mouse (0.5 µL in each side) over a 60-second period. The injection needle was not removed for an additional 60 seconds to facilitate the diffusion of the drugs (Houghoghi, Rezayof, Zyaian, & Zarrindast, 2009). 
2.5. Verifying cannula placement
The mice were anesthetized after the testing sessions and 0.5 μL/site of a 4% methylene-blue solution was infused into the CA1. Mice brains were removed after decapitation, and placed in 10% formaldehyde. Then, according to Paxinos and Watson, the selected CA1 areas were sliced and the sites of injections were verified (Paxinos & Franklin, 2001).
2.6. Passive avoidance apparatus
Behavioral procedures started 5 to 7 days after the surgery. The apparatus was a wooden box (30×30×40 cm high) and its bottom made of a series of parallel stainless steel bars (0.3 cm diameter spaced 1 cm apart). A wooden platform (4×4×4 cm) was located on the center of the grid floor. The animals were placed on the platform and their latency to step down the grid with 4 paws was measured during the training session. When the animals stepped down, electric shocks (1 Hz, 0.5 s, 45 V DC) were given continuously for 15 seconds. Animals spending more than 20 seconds on the platform or the ones that stepped up the platform before 15 seconds of electric shocks were eliminated from the experiments. Retention test (24 hours after training) was similar to training, but no shock was given to the animals. Latency to step-down was recorded and taken as a measure of memory retention. The upper cut off time was 180 seconds (Rezayof, Amini, Rassouli, & Zarrindast, 2006; Zarrindast and Rezayof, 2004). The retention test was performed between 8:00 AM and 2:00 PM.
2.7. Drug treatment
Each experimental group contained 10 mice. In the experimental group, each animal received 2 injections. All control groups received 2 injections of saline. 
2.7.1. Evaluation of passive avoidance memory retrieval following pre-training and pretest (Intra-CA1) administration of scopolamine 
In the current experiment, 4 groups (10 mice per group) of mice were examined. Control animals received 1 μL/mouse saline solution, whereas other groups were pretreated with scopolamine (0.75, 1.5, and 3 μg/mouse, Intra-CA1) 5 minutes before training. On the retrieval testing day, the control mice received saline solution (1 μL/mouse) 5 minutes prior to testing. Three other groups received the same doses of scopolamine (0.75, 1.5, and 3 μg/mouse, Intra-CA1) 5 minutes before testing. 
2.7.2. Evaluation of passive avoidance memory retrieval following pre-training and pretest intraperitoneally administration of morphine 
In this part of the experiment, 40 mice were divided into the following groups: Control group: mice received only vehicles (1 mL/kg); morphine treated groups received 
different doses (1, 3, and 5 mg/kg, i.p.) of morphine 30 minutes before training. On the test day, control mice received saline (1 mL/kg) 30 minutes prior to testing. But, other groups received different doses of morphine (1, 3, and 5 mg/kg, i.p.) 30 minutes before testing.
2.7.3. Assessment of pretest intraperitoneally morphine injection on memory retrieval impairment induced by Intra-CA1 injection of scopolamine
In this part, mice were divided into 2 control group that received saline (1 mL/mouse, Intra-CA1) before training, and saline (1 μL/mouse, Intra-CA1) 5 minutes before testing, and 3 treatment groups that received pretest morphine (1.5, 3, and 6 mg/kg, i.p.) 5 minutes before testing and pre-training scopolamine (3 μg/mouse, Intra-CA1).
2.7.4. Assessment of pretest (Intra-CA1) administration of scopolamine on morphine induced passive avoidance retrieval impairment
The mice were divided into the following groups: controls that received saline (1 μL/mouse, Intra-CA1) before training and after 24 hours of testing; control mice received saline (1 μL/mouse, Intra-CA1) 5 minutes before the onset of testing. Another 3 groups of animals received morphine (1, 3, and 6 mg/kg, i.p.) pre-training and 24 hours after training, they received saline (1 μL/mouse, Intra-CA1) or scopolamine (3 μg/mouse, Intra-CA1) 5 minutes before the onset of testing. 
2.8. Data analysis
Data (Step-down latencies) are expressed as mean and interquartile ranges. Large individual variations were observed in the step-down latency time. Data were analyzed with the Kruskal–Wallis nonparametric one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Comparisons between the paired groups were performed using the nonparametric Mann–Whitney U-test. For all statistical tests, P<0.05 was statistically significant.
3. Results
3.1. Effects of pretest Intra-CA1 administration of SCO in mice pre-trained under SCO 
Pre-training administration of different doses of SCO (0.75, 1.5, and 3 mg/mice, Intra-CA1) altered the memory retrieval significantly (the Kruskal–Wallis, non-parametric ANOVA, H (2)=27.18, P<0.01) on the test day, compared with that of saline-treated animals (Figure 1, left panel). In the present experiments, memory retrieval significantly (P<0.05) impaired after the 2 doses of SCO (1.5 and 3 μg/mouse, Intra-CA1) on the test day; although, there was no significant effect of 0.75 μg/mouse SCO on memory retrieval (Figure 1, left panel). The greatest effect was observed at 3 μg/mouse Intra-CA1 of SCO. Administration of SCO (3 μg/mouse, Intra-CA1) prior to training impaired memory retrieval on the test day (Figure 1, right panel), but pretest administered SCO (1.5 and 3 μg/mouse, Intra-CA1) restored it (SCO state-dependent memory) in mice (the Kruskal–Wallis non-parametric ANOVA, H (2)=21.99, P<0.001) (Figure 1, right panel).
3.2. Effect of pretest intraperitoneal injection of morphine in mice pre-trained under morphine
Morphine (1.5, 3, and 6 mg/kg, i.p.) administration prior to the test impaired the memory retrieval in mice pre-trained under morphine, compared with saline-treated animals (Figure 2, right panel). Nevertheless, the high doses of morphine (3 and 6 mg/kg, i.p.) induced significant impairment in memory retrieval on the test day (the Kruskal–Wallis non-parametric ANOVA, H (2)=18.98, P<0.01), the low dose of morphine (1.5 mg/kg, i.p.) had no significant effect on memory retrieval. The maximum response was obtained with 6 mg/kg of drug. Pre-training administration of morphine (6 mg/Kg, i.p.) impaired memory retrieval on the test day (Figure 2, left panel), but it was restored when morphine (6 mg/kg, i.p.) was administered as pretest treatment (morphine state-dependent memory) in mice (the Kruskal–Wallis, non-parametric ANOVA, H (2)=27.18, P<0.01).
AWT IMAGE

AWT IMAGE

AWT IMAGE
 
3.3. Effect of pretest administration of SCO on memory impairment induced by pretraining morphine and pretest morphine administration on pretraining SCO induced amnesia; a cross state dependency 
Pretest application of morphine (3 and 6 mg/kg, i.p.) could significantly (the Kruskal–Wallis nonparametric ANOVA, H (6)=121.34, P<0.05 and P<0.001, respectively) reverse the pertaining Intra-CA1 amnesic effect of SCO (3 μg/rat) (Figure 3, left panel). Similarly, pretest administration of SCO (3 μg/mice, Intra-CA1) reversed (the Kruskal–Wallis ANOVA, H (6)=31.20, P<0.001) the impairment of retention induced by pre-training injection morphine (6 mg/kg, i.p.) in mice (Figure 3, right panel).
4. Discussion 
The results of the current study showed that the pre-training Intra-CA1 injection of SCO could impair the memory retrieval of mice when tested 24 hours later in step-down passive avoidance task. The current study findings were in agreement with those of similar observations by Piri et al. (2013). They showed that pre-training Intra-CA1 administration of SCO could induce memory impairment in one-trial step-down inhibitory avoidance task in rats. In this respect, the essential role of dorsal hippocampus cholinergic afferents was shown when both acquisition and retention of avoidance learning were controlled in rodents (Hung, Lin, Liao, & Wang, 2004). Also, studies on genetically muscarinic receptors knock-out mice showed that different subtypes of muscarinic receptors participated in the regulation of normal hippocampal cognitive functions (Tzavara et al., 2003). 
The microdialysis studies on rats showed that release of acetylcholine developed from hippocampal nerve terminals after administration of SCO (Hiramatsu, Murasawa, Nabeshima, & Kameyama, 1998). This enhanced release of acetylcholine may be caused by positive feedback after the blockade of presynaptic autoreceptors and/or postsynaptic muscarinic receptors (Hiramatsu et al., 1998). Thus, acetylcholine release in dorsal hippocampus during 
pretest may provide a base to overcome the pre-training amnesic effect of muscarinic blockage on passive avoidance memory (Souza, Bruning, Acker,  Neto, & Nogueira, 2013, Mitsushima, Sano, & Takahashi, 2013).
The current study findings about memory impairing effects of morphine after pre-training administration were also supported by previous research. For example, Zarrindast et al., suggested that intraperitoneal pre-training administration of morphine to mice impaired memory formation in the step-down experiment (Zarrindast, Kangarlu-Haghighi, Khalilzadeh, & Fazli-Tabaei, 2006b). Such effects were mediated only via the activation of mu-opioid receptor (Shiigi, Takahashi, & Kaneto, 1990), and high levels of mu-opioid receptors were expressed in the hippocampal formation (De Oliveira et al., 2010). In addition, the role of hippocampus mu-opioid receptors in memory was recently confirmed by studying gene knock-out systems in mouse (Jang et al., 2003)
Finally, cross state-dependent effect was observed between morphine and SCO on retrieval of passive avoidance memories in mice. It seems that the bidirectional modulatory relationship between the cholinergic and opioids systems in hippocampus provide a base for such cross state-dependent effects in rodents (Zarrindast, Farahmandfar, Rostami, & Rezayof, 2006c). One suggested mechanism for explanation of cross state-dependent effect between morphine and scopolamine is that each system can simulate the effects of other systems on CA1 neurons or circuits. Previous studies on chick brain demonstrated that the mu-opioid receptors on cholinergic terminals in the hippocampus were normally under tonic inhibition by the endogenous opiate system (He, Chen, Wang, & Ma, 2008). 
The intracerebroventricular administration of a low dose of endomorphin-1 and an endogenous mu-opioid receptor agonist could improve the SCO-induced impairment in a short-term memory task in mice (Sakaguchi Koseki, Wakamatsu, & Matsumura, 2003). On the other hand, morphine could increase acetylcholine release from brainstem synapses by synaptic disinhibition (Zhu, Bowman, Baghdoyan, & Lydic, 2008). Altogether, it seems that a bidirectional relationship links the opioid and cholinergic systems at hippocampal level. Li et al., using the schedule-controlled responding technique in rats investigated the existence of a direct functional and neurochemical interaction between the mu-opioid receptor agonist morphine and the muscarinic cholinergic antagonist scopolamine (Li et al. 2010). Their results confirmed the functional bidirectional interaction between morphine and scopolamine for CA1-related memory functions (Nasehi et al., 2014). 
In an electrophysiological study, Kearns et al. (2001) showed the mu-opioid receptor-mediated presynaptic enhancement of muscarinic receptor mediated EPSPs in single hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Indeed, both scopolamine and morphine are alkaloid and show many functional and pharmacological similarities at intracellular level and their signaling cascade (Telegdy, Bagosi, & Jaszberenyi, 2014). Altogether, it seems that interaction between SCO and morphine can activate the same mechanisms in CA1 area and provide a basis for cross state-dependent learning mechanism. 
Another hypothesis to explain the cross state-dependent learning phenomenon between SCO and morphine is that these compounds can indirectly simulate the effects of each other on passive avoidance via modulation of GABAergic system at CA1 level. Studies showed that muscarinic receptor antagonists impair memory by indirect modulation of inhibitory GABAergic interneuron in hippocampus (Chang & Liang, 2012). Also, it is shown that mu-opioid receptor modulate GABA (γ-Aminobutyric acid) receptors in hippocampal CA1 synapses (McQuiston, 2007). Hippocampal gene expression profile also provided further evidence for possible involvement of GABA receptors in SCO-induced amnesia (Brouillette, Young, During, & Quirion, 2007). 
The results of electrophysiological studies also confirmed that specific classes of GABAergic interneurons are the main sources of endogenous opioids and muscarinic receptors in the hippocampus that innervate other types of interneurons (Giannopoulos & Papatheodoropoulos, 2007; Fukudom et al., 2004). Interconnections between cholinergic, opioidergic, and GABAergic neurotransmission are reported at CA1 region of guinea pigs (Favaroni Mendes & Menescal-de-Oliveira, 2008). Parsaei et al., also showed the role of GABAergic system of hippocampus in mediating the state-dependent learning and memory (Parsaei, Rangchiyan, Ahmadi, & Zarrindast, 2011). Therefore, it seems that GABAergic system of CA1 area can act as an interface between opioid and cholinergic systems in cross state-dependent learning (Rezayof, Razavi, Haeri-Rohani, Rassouli, & Zarrindast, 2007). Thus, a second suggested hypothesis is the existence of an indirect interaction between cholinergic and opioidergic systems at the level of CA1, which states that GABAergic interneurons can act as intermediate pathway for cross state-dependent learning between morphine and SCO. 
In summary, results of the current study showed that pretest intra CA1 injection of SCO fully reversed pre-training morphine-induced amnesia in step-down passive avoidance in mice and vice versa, which indicated the existence of cross state-dependency learning between morphine and SCO. To explain the findings, the following hypotheses are suggested: (i) SCO and morphine can affect and simulate the functions of each other via bidirectional interaction in CA1 area or (ii) SCO and morphine can simulate the effects of each other by indirect interactions via GABAergic system, and hence, shape a cross state-dependent memory effect between each other. 
Acknowledgments
The current study was financially supported by Shahed University, Tehran, Iran. Authors wish to thank Mr. Fariborz Didaran for his assistance in editing of the manuscripts.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declared no conflicts of interest.
 
References
Alger, B. E., Nagode, D. A., & Tang, A. H. (2014). Muscarinic cholinergic receptors modulate inhibitory synaptic rhythms in hippocampus and neocortex. Frontiers in Synaptic Neuroscience, 6. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2014.00018
 
Alijanpour, S., & Rezayof, A. (2013). Involvement of dorsal hippocampal and medial septal nicotinic receptors in cross state-dependent memory between WIN55, 212-2 and nicotine or ethanol in mice. Neuroscience, 245, 61–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.04.030
 
Antonova, E., Parslow, D., Brammer, M., Simmons, A., Williams, S., Dawson, G. R., et al. (2011). Scopolamine disrupts hippocampal activity during allocentric spatial memory in humans: an fMRI study using a virtual reality analogue of the Morris Water Maze. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 25(9), 1256–65. doi: 10.1177/0269881110379285
 
Brouillette, J., Young, D., During, M. G., & Quirion, R. (2007). Hippocampal gene expression profiling reveals the possible involvement of Homer1 and GABA(B) receptors in scopolamine-induced amnesia. Psychopharmacology, 102(6), 1978–89. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2007.04666.x
 
Carrigan, K. A., & Dykstra, L. A. (2007). Behavioral effects of morphine and cocaine in M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor-deficient mice. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 191(4), 985-93. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0671-1
 
Chang, S. D., & Liang, K. C. (2012). Roles of hippocampal GABAA and muscarinic receptors in consolidation of context memory and context–shock association in contextual fear conditioning: A double dissociation study. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 98(1), 17–24. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.04.004
 
De Oliveira, M. S. R., da Silva Fernandes, M. J., Scorza, F. A., Persike, D. S., Scorza, C. A., da Ponte, J. B., et al. (2010). Acute and chronic exercise modulates the expression of MOR opioid receptors in the hippocampal formation of rats. Brain Research Bulletin, 83(5), 278–83. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.07.009
 
Duka, T., Edelmann, V., Schutt, B., Dorow, R., & Fichte, K. (1992). Scopolamine-induced amnesia in humans: lack of effects of the benzodiazepine receptor antagonist -carboline ZK 93426. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 6(3), 382–8. doi: 10.1177/026988119200600307
 
Fanselow, M. S., & Dong, H. W. (2010). Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron, 65(1), 7–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031
 
Favaroni Mendes, L. A., & Menescal-de-Oliveira, L. (2008). Role of cholinergic, opioidergic and GABAergic neurotransmission of the dorsal hippocampus in the modulation of nociception in guinea pigs. Life Sciences, 83(19-20), 644–50. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2008.09.006
 
Fukudome, Y., Ohno-Shosaku, T., Matsui, M., Omori, Y., Fukaya, M., Tsubokawa, H., et al. (2004). Two distinct classes of muscarinic action on hippocampal inhibitory synapses: M2-mediated direct suppression and M1/M3-mediated indirect suppression through endocannabinoid signalling. European Journal of Neuroscience, 19(10), 2682–92. doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816x.2004.03384.x
 
Ghorbanalizadeh-Khalifeh-Mahaleh, B., Taheri, S., Sahebgharani, M., Rezayof, A., Haeri-Rohani, A., & Zarrindast, M. R. (2005). Intra-dorsal hippocampal microinjections of lithium and scopolamine induce a cross state-dependent learning in mice. Archive of Iranian Medicine, 11(6), 629-38. PMID: 18976034
 
Giannopoulos, P., & Papatheodoropoulos, C. (2013). Effects of μ-opioid receptor modulation on the hippocampal network activity of sharp wave and ripples. British Journal of Pharmacology, 168(5), 1146–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2012.02240.x
 
He, J., Chen, Y. M., Wang, J. H., & Ma, Y. Y. (2009). Effect of co-administration of morphine and cholinergic antagonists on y-maze spatial recognition memory retrieval and locomotor activity in mice. Zoological Research, 29(6), 613–20. doi: 10.3724/sp.j.1141.2008.06613
 
Hiramatsu, M., Murasawa, H., Nabeshima, T., & Kameyama, T. (1998). Effects of U-50,488H on SCO-, mecamylamine- and dizocilpine-induced learning and memory impairment in rats. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 284, 858–67. 
 
Houghoghi, V., Rezayof, A., Zyaian, S., & Zarrindast, M. R. (2009). Intradorsal hippocampal microinjection of lithium reverses morphine-induced impairment of memory in mice: interactions with dopamine receptor mechanism(s). Behavioural Pharmacology, 20(8), 680–7. doi: 10.1097/fbp.0b013e3283323c75
 
Hung, C. H., Lin, M. T., Liao, J. F., & Wang, J. J. (2004). Scopolamine-induced amnesia can be prevented by heat shock pretreatment in rats. Neuroscience Letters, 364(2), 63–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.02.074
 
Jafari-Sabet, M., & Jannat-Dastjerdi, I. (2009). Muscimol state-dependent memory: Involvement of dorsal hippocampal μ-opioid receptors. Behavioural Brain Research, 202(1), 5–10. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.010
 
Jafari-Sabet, M. (2011). Involvement of dorsal hippocampal muscarinic cholinergic receptors on muscimol state-depend
ent memory of passive avoidance in mice. Life Sciences, 88(25-26), 1136–41. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2011.04.013

Jamali-Raeufy, N., Nasehi, M., Ebrahimi-ghiri, M., & Zarrindast, M. R. (2011). Cross state-dependency of learning between WIN55, 212-2 and scopolamine in rat dorsal hippocampus. Neuroscience Letters, 491(3), 227–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.01.056

Jang, C. G., Lee, S. Y., Yoo, J. H., Yan, J. J., Song, D. K., Loh, H. H., et al. (2003). Impaired water maze learning performance in μ-opioid receptor knockout mice. Molecular Brain Research, 117(1), 68–72. doi: 10.1016/s0169-328x(03)00291-2

Kearns, I. R., Morton, R. A., Bulters, D. O., & Davies, C. H. (2001). Opioid receptor regulation of muscarinic acetylcholine receptor-mediated synaptic responses in the hippocampus. Neuropharmacology, 41(5), 565–73. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(01)00108-3

Klinkenberg, I., & Blokland, A. (2010). The validity of scopolamine as a pharmacological model for cognitive impairment: A review of animal behavioral studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(8), 1307–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.001

Leung, L. S., & Peloquin, P. (2009). Cholinergic modulation differs between basal and apical dendritic excitation of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells. Cerebral Cortex, 20(8), 1865–77. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp251

Li, X., Li, J. X., & France, C. P. (2010). Interactions between morphine, scopolamine and nicotine: Schedule-controlled responding in rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 96(1), 91–5. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.04.011

Lo, A. C., De Maeyer, J. H., Vermaercke, B., Callaerts-Vegh, Z., Schuurkes, J. A., & D’Hooge, R. (2014). SSP-002392, a new 5-HT4 receptor agonist, dose-dependently reverses scopolamine-induced learning and memory impairments in C57Bl/6 mice. Neuropharmacology 85, 178-89. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.05.013

Mahmoodi, G., Ahmadi, S., pourmotabbed, A., Oryan, S., & Zarrindast, M. R. (2010). Inhibitory avoidance memory deficit induced by scopolamine: Interaction of cholinergic and glutamatergic systems in the ventral tegmental area. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 94(1), 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.04.004

Mitsushima, D., Sano, A., & Takahashi, T. (2013). A cholinergic trigger drives learning-induced plasticity at hippocampal synapses. Nature Communications, 4. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3760

McQuiston, A. R. (2007). Effects of opioid receptor modulation on GABAB receptor synaptic function in hippocampal CA1. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97(3), 2301–11. doi: 10.1152/jn.01179.2006

Nasehi, M., Nasehi, M., Rahmani-Nia, F., Mirzaei, B., Torabi-Nami, M., & Zarrindast, M. R. (2014). Swimming improves the emotional memory deficit by scopolamine via mu opioid receptors. Physiology & Behavior, 128, 237–46. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.02.011

Parsaei, L., Rangchiyan, M., Ahmadi, S., & Zarrindast, M. R. (2011). GABAA receptors in the dorsal hippocampus are involved in sate-dependent learning induced by lithium in mice. Iranian Journal of Pharmaceutical Research, 10(1), 127-34. PMCID: PMC3869577

Patti, C. L., Kameda, S. R., Carvalho, R. C., Takatsu-Coleman, A. L., Lopez, G. B., Niigaki, S. T., et al. (2005). Effects of morphine on the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task: Role of state-dependent learning. Psychopharmacology, 184(1), 1–12. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0238-6

Paxinos, G., & Franklin, K. B. J. (2001). The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates (2nd Ed.) New York: Academic Press.

Petersen, R. C. (1979). Scopolamine state-dependent memory processes in man. Psychopharmacology, 64(3), 309–14. doi: 10.1007/bf00427515

Piri, M., Rostampour, M., Nasehi, M., & Zarrindast, M. R. (2013). Blockade of the dorsal hippocampal dopamine D1 receptors inhibits the scopolamine-induced state-dependent learning in rats. Neuroscience, 252, 460–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.08.003

Ramakrishnan, L., Amatya, C., DeSaer, C. J., Dalhoff, Z., & Eggerichs, M. R. (2014). Galantamine reverses scopolamine-induced behavioral alterations in Dugesia tigrina. Invertebrate Neuroscience, 14(2), 91–101. doi: 10.1007/s10158-013-0167-8

Rezayof, A., Amini, R., Rassouli, Y., & Zarrindast, M. R. (2006). Influence of nitric oxide on morphine-induced amnesia and interactions with dopaminergic receptor agents. Physiology & Behavior, 88(1-2), 124–31. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.017

Rezayof, A., Razavi, S., Haeri-Rohani, A., Rassouli, Y., & Zarrindast, M. R. (2007). GABAA receptors of hippocampal CA1 regions are involved in the acquisition and expression of morphine-induced place preference. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 17(1), 24–31. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2006.02.003

Roldán, G., Cobos-Zapiain, G., Quirarte, G. L., & Prado-Alcalá, R. A. (2001). Dose- and time-dependent scopolamine-induced recovery of an inhibitory avoidance response after its extinction in rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 121(1-2), 173–9. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00157-7

Sakaguchi, M., Koseki, M., Wakamatsu, M., & Matsumura, E. (2003). Effects of β-Casomorphin-5 on Passive Avoidance Response in Mice. Bioscience, Biotechnology, and Biochemistry, 67(11), 2501–4. doi: 10.1271/bbb.67.2501

Shiigi, Y., Takahashi, M., & Kaneto, H. (1990). Facilitation of memory retrieval by pretest morphine mediated by μ but not δ and κ opioid receptors. Psychopharmacology, 102(3), 329–32. doi: 10.1007/bf02244099

Souza, A. C. G., Bruning, C. A., Acker, C. I., Neto, J. S. S., & Nogueira, C. W. (2013). 2-Phenylethynyl-butyltellurium enhances learning and memory impaired by scopolamine in mice. Behavioural Pharmacology, 24(4), 249–54. doi: 10.1097/fbp.0b013e32836353a5

Telegdy, G., Bagosi, Z., & Jaszberenyi, M. (2014). Transmitter-mediated action of neuromedin S on Passive avoidance learning in rats. Journal of Neurobehavioral Sciences, 1(2), 41. doi: 10.5455/jnbs.1398170234

Tzavara, E. T., Bymaster, F. P., Felder, C. C., Wade, M., Gomeza, J., Wess, J., et al. (2003). Dysregulated hippocampal acetylcholine neurotransmission and impaired cognition in M2, M4 and M2/M4 muscarinic receptor knockout mice. Molecular Psychiatry, 8(7), 673–9. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001270

Vega-Flores, G., Gruart, A., & Delgado-García, J. M. (2014). Involvement of the GABAergic Septo-Hippocampal Pathway 
in Brain Stimulation Reward. PLoS ONE, 9(11), 113787. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113787

Whitlock, J. R. (2006). Learning Induces Long-Term Potentiation in the Hippocampus. Science, 313(5790), 1093–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1128134

Xiang, X. H., Wang, H. L., Wu, W. R., Guo, Y., Cao, D. Y., Wang, H. S., et al. (2006). Ethological analysis of scopolamine treatment or pretreatment in morphine dependent rats. Physiology & Behavior, 88(1-2), 183–90. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.029

Young, A. M. (2014). State-dependent learning and memory. In I. P. Stolerman (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Psychopharmacology (pp. 1648-1659). Philadelphia: Springer.

Zarrindast, M. R., & Rezayof, A. (2004). Morphine state-dependent learning: sensitization and interactions with dopamine receptors. European Journal of Pharmacology, 497(2), 197–204. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2004.06.041

Zarrindast, M. R., Fazli-Tabaei, S., Ahmadi, S., & Yahyavi, S. H. (2006a). Effect of lithium on morphine state-dependent memory of passive avoidance in mice. Physiology & Behavior, 87(2), 409–15. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.11.005

Zarrindast, M. R., Kangarlu-Haghighi, K., Khalilzadeh, A., & Fazli-Tabaei, S. (2006b). Influence of intracerebroventricular administration of cannabinergic drugs on morphine state-dependent memory in the step-down passive avoidance test. Behavioural Pharmacology, 17(3), 231–7. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200605000-00004

Zarrindast, M. R., Farahmandfar, M., Rostami, P., & Rezayof, A. (2006c). The influence of central administration of dopaminergic and cholinergic agents on morphine-induced amnesia in morphine-sensitized mice. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 20(1), 59–66. doi: 10.1177/0269881105057003

Zarrindast, M. R., Ardjmand, A., Rezayof, A., & Ahmadi, S. (2013). The time profile of morphine effect on different phases of inhibitory avoidance memory in rat. Archive of Iranian Medicine, 16(1), 34-7. doi: 013161/AIM.0011

Zarrindast, M. R., Ownegh, V., Rezayof, A., & Ownegh, F. (2014). The involvement of dorsal hippocampus in dextromethorphan-induced state-dependent learning in mice. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 116, 90–5. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.11.015

Zhai, H., Wu, P., Chen, S., Li, F., Liu, Y., & Lu, L. (2008). Effects of scopolamine and ketamine on reconsolidation of morphine conditioned place preference in rats. Behavioural Pharmacology, 19(3), 211–6. doi: 10.1097/fbp.0b013e3282fe88a0

Zhu, Z., Bowman, H. R., Baghdoyan, H. A., & Lydic, R. (2008). Morphine increases acetylcholine release in the trigeminal nuclear complex. Sleep, 31(12), 1629–37. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.12.1629
Type of Study: Original | Subject: Behavioral Neuroscience
Received: 2016/08/30 | Accepted: 2016/11/19 | Published: 2017/05/1

Add your comments about this article : Your username or Email:
CAPTCHA

Send email to the article author


Rights and permissions
Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

© 2024 CC BY-NC 4.0 | Basic and Clinical Neuroscience

Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb